Information

  • Author Services

Initiatives

You are accessing a machine-readable page. In order to be human-readable, please install an RSS reader.

All articles published by MDPI are made immediately available worldwide under an open access license. No special permission is required to reuse all or part of the article published by MDPI, including figures and tables. For articles published under an open access Creative Common CC BY license, any part of the article may be reused without permission provided that the original article is clearly cited. For more information, please refer to https://www.mdpi.com/openaccess .

Feature papers represent the most advanced research with significant potential for high impact in the field. A Feature Paper should be a substantial original Article that involves several techniques or approaches, provides an outlook for future research directions and describes possible research applications.

Feature papers are submitted upon individual invitation or recommendation by the scientific editors and must receive positive feedback from the reviewers.

Editor’s Choice articles are based on recommendations by the scientific editors of MDPI journals from around the world. Editors select a small number of articles recently published in the journal that they believe will be particularly interesting to readers, or important in the respective research area. The aim is to provide a snapshot of some of the most exciting work published in the various research areas of the journal.

Original Submission Date Received: .

  • Active Journals
  • Find a Journal
  • Proceedings Series
  • For Authors
  • For Reviewers
  • For Editors
  • For Librarians
  • For Publishers
  • For Societies
  • For Conference Organizers
  • Open Access Policy
  • Institutional Open Access Program
  • Special Issues Guidelines
  • Editorial Process
  • Research and Publication Ethics
  • Article Processing Charges
  • Testimonials
  • Preprints.org
  • SciProfiles
  • Encyclopedia

education-logo

Article Menu

line dance research paper

  • Subscribe SciFeed
  • Recommended Articles
  • Google Scholar
  • on Google Scholar
  • Table of Contents

Find support for a specific problem in the support section of our website.

Please let us know what you think of our products and services.

Visit our dedicated information section to learn more about MDPI.

JSmol Viewer

Effects of line dancing on mental health in seniors after covid-19 pandemic.

line dance research paper

1. Introduction

2.1. design and participants, 2.2. training protocol, 2.3. data collection, 2.4. statistical analysis, 4. discussion, 5. conclusions, author contributions, institutional review board statement, informed consent statement, data availability statement, conflicts of interest.

  • Capone, R. Ballare Danzare. Teoria e Tecnica di Danza Sportiva. Regole Generali e Consigli Pratici ; Gremese Editore: Rome, Italy, 2006; p. 200. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Nied, R.J.; Franklin, B. Promoting and prescribing exercise for the elderly. Am. Fam. Physician 2002 , 65 , 419–426. [ Google Scholar ] [ PubMed ]
  • American College of Sports Medicine Position Stand. Exercise and physical activity for older adults. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 1998 , 30 , 992–1008. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Vita, A.J.; Terry, R.B.; Hubert, H.B.; Fries, J.F. Aging, health risks, and cumulative disability. N. Engl. J. Med. 1998 , 338 , 1035–1041. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ] [ Green Version ]
  • Istat. Le Condizioni di Salute Della Popolazione Anziana in Italia. Anno 2019. Available online: https://www.istat.it/it/files//2021/07/Report-anziani-2019.pdf (accessed on 5 October 2021).
  • WHO. Ageing and Health. 4 October 2021. Available online: https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/ageing-and-health (accessed on 5 October 2021).
  • WHO. Physical Activity Strategy for the WHO European Region 2016–2025 ; WHO Regional Office for Europe: Vilnus, Lithuania, 2015. Available online: https://www.euro.who.int/__data/assets/pdf_file/0010/282961/65wd09e_PhysicalActivityStrategy_150474.pdf (accessed on 5 October 2021).
  • Bennett, C.G.; Hackney, M.E. Effects of line dancing on physical function and perceived limitation in older adults with self-reported mobility limitations. Disabil. Rehabil. 2018 , 40 , 1259–1265. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Nadasen, K. Life without line dancing and the other activities would be too dreadful to imagine: An increase in social activity for older women. J. Women Aging 2008 , 20 , 329–342. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Altavilla, G.; D’ Elia, F.; Raiola, G. A brief review of the effects of physical activity in subjects with cardiovascular disease: An interpretative key. Sport Mont. 2018 , 16 , 103–106. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • D’ Isanto, T.; Manna, A.; Altavilla, G. Health and physical activity. Sport Sci. 2017 , 10 , 100–105. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Behrman, S.; Ebmeier, K.P. Can exercise prevent cognitive decline? Practitioner 2014 , 258 , 17–21. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dawn, J.; Southcott, J. Meanings of leisure for older people: An Australian study of line dancing. Leis. Stud. 2019 , 38 , 74–87. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Barry, L.C.; Allore, H.G.; Guo, Z.; Bruce, M.L.; Gill, T.M. Higher burden of depression among older women: The effect of onset, persistence, and mortality over time. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 2008 , 65 , 172–178. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Thielke, S.M.; Diehr, P.; Unutzer, J. Prevalence, incidence, and persistence of major depressive symptoms in the Cardiovascular Health Study. Aging Ment. Health 2010 , 14 , 168–176. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • World Health Organization. Age-Friendly Primary Health Care Centre Toolkit ; World Health Organization: Geneva, Switzerland, 2008. [ Google Scholar ]
  • La Sorveglianza Passi d’Argento. PASSI and PASSI d’Argento and COVID-19 Pandemic. 2020. Available online: https://www.epicentro.iss.it/passi-argento/ (accessed on 5 October 2021).
  • Pantell, M.; Rehkopf, D.; Jutte, D.; Syme, S.L.; Balmes, J.; Adler, N. Social isolation: A predictor of mortality comparable to traditional clinical risk factors. Am. J. Public Health 2013 , 103 , 2056–2062. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Raiola, G.; Aliberti, S. Outdoor sports and physical activity during social distancing by sports sciences and exercise course students at the University of Salerno. J. Phys. Educ. Sport 2021 , 21 , 612–617. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Raiola, G.; Aliberti, S.; Esposito, G.; Altavilla, G.; D’ Isanto, T.; D’ Elia, F. How has the practice of physical activity changed during the covid-19 quarantine? A preliminary survey. Teorìâ Ta Metod. Fìzičnogo Vihovannâ 2020 , 20 , 242–247. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Sannicandro, I.; Cofano, G.; Rosa, A.R.; Raiola, G. Sedentary conditions during the lockdown and movement opportunities for the Italian elderly. MOJ Sports Med. 2020 , 4 , 87–90. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lee, K.; Jeong, G.C.; Yim, J. Consideration of the Psychological and Mental Health of the Elderly during COVID-19: A Theoretical Review. Int J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020 , 17 , 8098. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Bai, Y.; Lin, C.C.; Lin, C.Y.; Chen, J.Y.; Chue, C.M.; Chou, P. Survey of stress reactions among health care workers involved with the SARS outbreak. Psychiatr. Serv. 2004 , 55 , 1055–1057. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Brooks, S.K.; Webster, R.K.; Smith, L.E.; Woodland, L.; Wessely, S.; Greenberg, N.; James Rubin, G. The psychological impact of quarantine and how to reduce it: Rapid review of the evidence. Lancet 2020 , 395 , 912–920. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ Green Version ]
  • Santini, Z.I.; Jose, P.E.; Cornwell, E.Y.; Koyanagi, A.; Nielsen, L.; Hinrichsen, C.; Meilstrup, C.; Madsen, K.R.; Koushede, V. Social disconnectedness, perceived isolation, and symptoms of depression and anxiety among older Americans (NSHAP): A longitudinal mediation analysis. Lancet Public Health 2020 , 5 , 62–70. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ Green Version ]
  • D’Elia, F.; D’Isanto, T. Outdoor movement education in primary school during COVID-19 pandemic in the synthetic perceptions of primary school university training student. J. Hum. Sport Exerc. 2021 , 16 , 1522–1537. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • D’Elia, F. Inclusion in physical and sport education for special movement needs. J. Hum. Sport Exerc. 2021 , 16 , 781–787. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Akandere, M.; Demir, B. The effect of dance over depression. Coll Antropol. 2011 , 35 , 651–656. [ Google Scholar ] [ PubMed ]
  • Hernandez, R.; Andrade, F.C.D.; Piedra, L.M.; Tabb, K.M.; Xu, S.; Sarkisian, C. The impact of exercise on depressive symptoms in older Hispanic/Latino adults: Results from the ‘Caminemos’ study. Aging Ment. Health 2019 , 23 , 680–685. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Penninx, B.W.; Leveille, S.; Ferrucci, L.; Eijk, J.T.; Guralnik, J.M. Exploring the effect of depression on physical disability: Longitudinal evidence from the established populations for epidemiologic studies of the elderly. Am. J. Public Health 1999 , 89 , 1346–1352. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ Green Version ]
  • Sheikh, J.I.; Yesavage, J.A. Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS): Recent evidence and development of a shorter version. Clin. Gerontol. 1986 , 5 , 165–173. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Yesavage, J.A.; Brink, T.L.; Rose, T.L.; Lum, O.; Huang, V.; Adey, M.; Leirer, V.O. Development and validation of a geriatric depression screening scale: A preliminary report. J. Psychiatr. Res. 1982 , 17 , 37–49. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Cohen, J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences , 2nd ed.; Lawrence Earlbaum Associates: Hillsdale, CA, USA, 1988. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cooper, L.; Thomas, H. Growing old gracefully: Social dance in the third age. Ageing Soc. 2002 , 22 , 689–708. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Paulson, S.M. An Exploration of How Various ‘Cultures of Dance’ Construct Experiences of Health and Growing Older. Ph.D. Thesis, City University London, London, UK, 2009. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Murrock, C.J.; Graor, C.H. Effects of dance on depression, physical function, and disability in underserved adults. J. Aging Phys. Act. 2014 , 22 , 380–385. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Vankova, H.; Holmerova, I.; Machacova, K.; Volicer, L.; Veleta, P.; Celko, A.M. The effect of dance on depressive symptoms in nursing home residents. J. Am. Med. Dir. Assoc. 2014 , 15 , 582–587. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Raiola, G. Inclusion in sport dance and self perception. Sport Sci. 2015 , 8 , 99–102. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Overdorf, V.; Kollia, B.; Makarec, K.; Alleva Szeles, C. The Relationship Between Physical Activity and Depressive Symptoms in Healthy Older Women. Gerontol. Geriatr. Med. 2016 , 2 , 2333721415626859. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Gerlach, L.; Solway, E.; Singer, D.; Kullgren, J.; Kirch, M.; Malani, P. Mental Health Among Older Adults Before and During the COVID-19 Pandemic. Univ. Mich. Natl. Poll Healthy Aging 2021 . [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Morgan, G.S.; Willmott, M.; Ben-Shlomo, Y.; Haase, A.M.; Campbell, R.M. A life fulfilled: Positively influencing physical activity in older adults—A systematic review and meta-ethnography. BMC Public Health 2019 , 19 , 362. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ] [ Green Version ]
ItemYesNo
01
10
10
10
01
10
01
10
10
10
01
10
01
10
10
Cases Score PreScore Post
Dancer 186
Dancer 2107
Dancer 386
Dancer 475
Dancer 596
Dancer 61410
Dancer 784
Dancer 886
Dancer 9127
Dancer 10116
Dancer 1185
Dancer 12119
Dancer 1375
Dancer 1497
RangePrePost
Possible presence of depression10–15 points51
Possible depression6–9 points99
Improbable depression0–5 points 04
Paired DifferencestglSign. (Two Tails)
MeanStandard DeviationStandard Error of Mean95% Confidence Interval of Difference
LowerUpper
GDSPre-post2.928571.141140.304982.269703.587459.602130.000
GDS Item Mean ± SDCohen’s d
PrePostD ValueEffect Size (ES)
0.8 ± 0.30.5 ± 0.50.6Moderate
1 ± 00.4 ± 0.51Large
0.4 ± 0.50.2 ± 0.40.2Small
1 ± 00.5 ± 0.50.8Large
0.8 ± 0.30.4 ± 0.50.8Large
0.2 ± 0.40.3 ± 0.4-0.1Ignored
0.7 ± 0.40.5 ± 0.40.7Moderate
0.7 ± 0.40.6 ± 0.40.1Ignored
0.1 ± 0.300.3Small
0.5 ± 0.50.5 ± 0.50Ignored
0.7 ± 0.40.5 ± 0.50.5Moderate
0.3 ± 0.40.3 ± 0.40Ignored
0.3 ± 0.40.2 ± 0.40.3Small
0.3 ± 0.40.2 ± 0.40.1Ignored
0.8 ± 0.30.8 ± 0.30Ignored
MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

Aliberti, S.; Raiola, G. Effects of Line Dancing on Mental Health in Seniors after COVID-19 Pandemic. Educ. Sci. 2021 , 11 , 677. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci11110677

Aliberti S, Raiola G. Effects of Line Dancing on Mental Health in Seniors after COVID-19 Pandemic. Education Sciences . 2021; 11(11):677. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci11110677

Aliberti, Sara, and Gaetano Raiola. 2021. "Effects of Line Dancing on Mental Health in Seniors after COVID-19 Pandemic" Education Sciences 11, no. 11: 677. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci11110677

Article Metrics

Article access statistics, further information, mdpi initiatives, follow mdpi.

MDPI

Subscribe to receive issue release notifications and newsletters from MDPI journals

Last updated 10th July 2024: Online ordering is currently unavailable due to technical issues. We apologise for any delays responding to customers while we resolve this. For further updates please visit our website https://www.cambridge.org/news-and-insights/technical-incident

We use cookies to distinguish you from other users and to provide you with a better experience on our websites. Close this message to accept cookies or find out how to manage your cookie settings .

Login Alert

line dance research paper

  • < Back to search results
  • Dance Research Journal

Dance Research Journal

  • Submit your article
  • Announcements

DRJ society logo 2018

This journal utilises an Online Peer Review Service (OPRS) for submissions. By clicking "Continue" you will be taken to our partner site https://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/drj . Please be aware that your Cambridge account is not valid for this OPRS and registration is required. We strongly advise you to read all "Author instructions" in the "Journal information" area prior to submitting.

  • Information
  • Journal home
  • Journal information
  • Open access articles
  • Latest issue
  • Get access Subscribe Check if you have access via personal or institutional login Log in Register
  • Contains open access

Dance Research Journal

  • ISSN: 0149-7677 (Print) , 1940-509X (Online)
  • Editors: Nadine George-Graves Northwestern University, USA , and Rebekah Kowal The University of Iowa, USA
  • Editorial board

Latest content

Editor's note.

  • Rebekah J. Kowal
  • Dance Research Journal , Volume 55 , Issue 3

Tidalectic Un/mapping and the Performance of African Diasporic Imagination in the Repertory of Katherine Dunham

  • Tia-Monique Uzor

Coppélia 's Human-Objects: Winding Up Racialized Automata on the Ballet Stage

  • Mara Mandradjieff

A “Tick and Flick” Exercise: Movement and Form in Australian Parliamentary Human Rights Scrutiny

  • Sean Mulcahy , Kate Seear

Is Mademoiselle Mercédès Always Julienne Mathieu? The Challenges of Using a Stage Name to Reconstruct the Career of a Parisian Belle Époque Music Hall Dancer

  • Mercedes Alvarez San Román

Leading the Other: Gender and Colonialism in Partner Dancing's Long Century

  • David Kaminsky

JAY PATHER, PERFORMANCE, AND SPATIAL POLITICS IN SOUTH AFRICA by Ketu H. Katrak. 2021. Bloomington: Indiana University Press. 436 pp., 30 illustrations. $35 paper, ISBN-10: 9780253053684, ISBN-13: 9780253053688.

  • Rosemary Candelario

DANCE IN US POPULAR CULTURE edited by Jennifer Atkins. 2023. London: Routledge. 358 pp., 30 b/w illustrations. $37.56 paper and e-book, ISBN: 9780367819842, ISBN: 9781003011170. $136.00 hardcover, ISBN: 9780367819729. doi: 10.4324/9781003011170.

  • Hannah Schwadron

Music, Theatre and Art on fifteeneightyfour

Listening to the unexpected: monteverdi and the marvellous.

  • 11 December 2023, Roseen Giles
  • How do we learn to listen? Like most worthwhile things, listening well takes time, practice, and perseverance. While it might seem like good music ought to The post Listening to the Unexpected: Monteverdi and the Marvellous first appeared on Fifteen Eighty Four | Cambridge University Press....

Giotto’s Scrovegni Chapel in the Arena of History

  • 04 December 2023, Henrike Christiane Lange
  • Giotto’s Arena Chapel and the Triumph of Humility takes its lead from three features of the famous monument that each engage the question of time, material, The post Giotto’s Scrovegni Chapel in the Arena of History first appeared on Fifteen Eighty Four | Cambridge University Press....

Performance, Modernity and the Plays

  • 26 October 2023, Hélène Lecossois
  • Why engage with a canonical playwright? Isn’t there enough work to do trying and recovering the works of playwrights who have all but been erased from the canon The post Performance, Modernity and the Plays first appeared on Fifteen Eighty Four | Cambridge University Press....

Literature & Performance on Twitter

Dance Research Journal - sign up for Alerts!

The Body as Archive: Will to Re-Enact and the Afterlives of Dances

  • André Lepecki
  • Dance Research Journal , Volume 42 , Issue 2

COE share button

  •    Home

Line dance is an international sport which brings physical and mental pleasure by repeating fixed steps accompanied by music. It takes music as its core and shows the multi-cultural charm of folk dances of different countries around the world through the combination and cycle of dance steps with different styles. Line dance is a new type of sports dance. It is still in the initial stage in sports teaching of colleges and universities. Therefore, in this paper college students’ physical, musical and psychological qualities are improved through an experiment, which further shows that college students’ mastering of skills of line dance has positive significance.

Strengthen College Students , Line Dance , Skill Elements

Share and Cite:

line dance research paper

1. Introduction

Line dance is a new international sport dance. The specific content of this kind of dance is as follows. First of all, we need to select music, match dance steps with suitable music, and maintain free standing positions. Whether in a row or in a circle, we can achieve the popular dance fitness participated by body which combines music, dance and other artistic forms. At the same time, the relevant people participating in dance are required to follow the prescribed dance movements. Each piece of dance music of line dance basically has its own rhythm, and varies by rhythm. It can be found in the teaching practice that many college students can complete dance movements in the normal physical education course and master the skills of line dance in repeated training after the public physical cause is offered in colleges and universities. Some of the students’ line dance movements are not standard enough due to their lack of strength, speed and coordination ability. The main reasons include the poor sense of rhythm of music, lack of confidence in dance, rigid body movements and so on. However, in fact line dance can effectively strengthen college students’ physical function, cultivate their music literacy and enhance their psychological quality. Therefore, this paper further strengthens college students’ line dance skills through the study of these three elements.

2. Research Objects and Methods

2.1. Research Objects

Two classes of college students in a university were taken as the research objects. Among them, class A was used as the experimental object, and class B was used as the control experimental group. Each class had 30 students. Basic conditions of subjects (see Table 1 ).

In the experiment, the questionnaire and statistical analysis method were used for study, and both classes had some dance experience. Such experiment needs to take a semester as the experimental time. A sports course of line dance is offered once a week, and line dance sports are carried out normally according to the time of college sports course. Before the experiment, we needed to collect the results of the physical quality and health test of Class A and Class B, and tried to keep the basic physical quality and physical ability level of the two groups consistent [1] .

2.2. Research Contents

The main contents of the experiment include three aspects. First, the physical quality. The control group only did some stretching exercises according to the original teaching method, learned normally and repeated training according to the teacher’s dance steps, and relaxed properly at the end of the experiment. The experimental group warmed up by leg-raising and jogging in the warm-up stage, which could improve coordination ability of the body. Then, according to the basketball training movements, they ran back and forth, followed by line dance [2] . Basically, each person needed to complete several groups of sit-up and relax after completion.

Second, in terms of music training, the control group needed to play the dance music in line dance according to the routine situation, but the students’ sense of music rhythm should be observed, and students needed to imitate constantly, so as to better follow the music rhythm. In teaching in the experimental group, the style and rhythm of music were analyzed, and training of the rhythm of music was provided constantly, so as to help students better grasp the rhythm of music and experience the mood of music in many dance movements.

Table 1 . Basic conditions of experimental subjects.

Finally, in terms of psychological quality, normal teaching was still provided to the control group. After completing the teaching task, the problems of students existing in the training were corrected. In addition to the normal evaluation, no other training was provided. In the experimental group, training was provided to students by group after the completion of the teaching. At the same time, a number of simulation competitions were made. At the same time, students needed to correct and evaluate their own training results [3] . Students need to be encouraged in training, so that they could perform in the community, gradually accumulate more performance experience, and have better psychological quality.

After a semester of experiment, the physical quality of the two groups of students was evaluated. The specific indicators of this evaluation meet the requirements of the Ministry of Education for students’ physical education evaluation. According to the relevant requirements of students’ health and physique, the results of the evaluation were obtained scientifically and reasonably. In addition, a questionnaire survey should be made. According to the actual cognitive level of students in line dance and rhythm training, the survey was made to find the effect of mastering of this sense of rhythm, and check whether the students’ physical quality had changed. The specific test content includes students’ feeling of body change, sports skills, actual physical conditions, whether the student is strong or not, etc. The main result of this questionnaire survey is the satisfaction of college students to their physical conditions. Then, a dance song was selected as the most important music used in the examination. The final evaluation was made by fixed judges on two groups of students [4] .

3. Research Results

3.1. The Effect of Improvement of Physical Quality on College Students’ Mastering of Skills of Line Dance

Comparison of Physical Quality between Control Group and Experimental Group

Adoption of Table 2 , it could be found that there was no significant difference in physical quality and ability between the control group and the experimental group before the experiment, which basically met the requirements of the experiment. After a semester of training, the physical quality of two classes improved significantly, but comparing the two groups it could be found that the experimental group (Group A) had more obvious physical improvement. In the physical training such as 800 running, long jump and sit-up, it could be found that the physical quality of the experimental group had been significantly improved, so we could basically conclude that in line dance increasing the content of physical quality training could significantly improve the physical quality of students, so it has a positive effect [5] .

3.2. The Effect of Improvement of Music Literacy on College Students’ Mastering of Skills of Line Dance

Statistical Results of Post-Experiment Questionnaire in Control Group and Experimental Group

So the results of the survey in Table 3 show that it could be found that many college students admitted that music could effectively improve their skills in practice of line dance. The focuses were the need for rhythm training and the improvement of the performance of line dance by understanding of music. It was obvious that the experimental group was more supportive, while the control group was less supportive. After the experiment, the line dance test on college students continued. The results of the related tests showed that the experimental group to which music knowledge was explained could have a deeper understanding of music and a better sense of rhythm. In addition, the experimental group also had bigger expressive force and tension, and good aesthetic feeling. In conclusion, the improvement of music literacy is of great significance for college students to master the skills of line dance.

Table 2 . Changes of physical fitness index of female college students before and after the experiment.

Table 3 . Investigation on music appreciation ability and rhythm training of college students.

3.3. The Effect of Improvement of Psychological Quality on College Students’ Mastering of Skills of Line Dance

At present, the mental health of college students has been paid attention by the society. To a great extent, mental health directly affects the healthy growth of college students. In the stage of college students, teenagers change into adults. Facing many psychological changes, there are great moral problems in self-psychological adjustment and control caused by lack of experience. Therefore, they lack adjustment ability when facing economic and emotional pressures, leading to serious psychological impact. Therefore, it is easy to have very intense psychological contradictions. By comparing the test results before and after the beginning of the experiment between the control group and the experimental group, it could be found that students’ physical self-esteem and physical attraction improved significantly [6] . However, comparing the training results of the control group and the experimental group, it could be found that the experimental group was better than the control group in all the items, and the experimental group had a more significant improvement. This can show that the psychological quality of the students in the experimental group significantly improved.

Finally, the results of college students’ line dance test were compared. In the results, it could be found that in the items of college students’ line dance sports such as movement proficiency, opening modeling and performance in line dance the psychological quality of the experimental group had significantly improved, so the results of the experimental group were obviously better than those of the control group. Therefore, we can see that in the evaluation of line dance the experimental group was more confident and had bigger expressive force. In actual training, their movements would not be uncoordinated because they were too nervous. In the line dance performance of the experimental group, they showed better vitality, and the performance was more fluent. Students were obviously relaxed, and had better aesthetic feeling. Through these experiments, it can be found that good psychological quality can effectively help students complete the line dance, so that they can really integrate body and mind into the dance movements and show better results.

4. Conclusions and Recommendations

Line dance is mainly characterized by the popularity of national fitness. Colleges and universities carry out sports activities, in order to cultivate students’ comprehensive abilities in addition to knowledge of cultural courses. From the perspective of the type of bodybuilding exercise, line dance is simple and easy to learn. As long as you follow the music, you can do exercise. Second, line dance has various styles, a lot of contents and many elements of national dance, thus forming a new form of line dance. Line dance has such functions as teaching, body building and pleasure in temperament. First of all, in terms of teaching function, as a new type of mass fitness activity, line dance can be actively carried out in college sports activities. In the healthy physical fitness activities, it plays an important role in promoting the physical education teaching in China. It can improve the body coordination of practitioners and speed up the body circulation. In addition, it can also achieve pleasure in temperament, make learners affected by music, make practitioners closer, and cultivate people’s sentiments [7] . Therefore, it has certain positive significance to carry out line dance in colleges and universities.

This paper divided college students into two groups for experiment. By observing the experimental results, it can be found that in line dance teaching proper line dance can improve the physical quality of college students. Compared with simple exercise activities, it can better improve the physical quality of college students. Music practice can make college students better understand music, so that they can combine music in line dance, better integrate music and dance steps, and perform more skillfully in the performance. In addition, in the continuous simulation training students’ psychological endurance ability to the competition can be improved, and the psychological quality ability can be further improved, so under the strong self-confidence college students can have bigger expressive force in the line dance. To sum up, psychological quality, music quality and physical function can effectively promote college students to master the skills of line dance. They promote each other, so they should be developed in an all-round way.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest regarding the publication of this paper.

[ ] Huang, T., Xiao, H., Fan, L., et al. (2017) Research on the Impact of Line Dance on Health and Physical Fitness of Non-PE Major Female College Students. Neijiang Sci-ence and Technology, 38, 108-109.
[ ] Fan, T. (2018) Investigation and Research on Psychological Motivation of Line Dance Learning of Students in Shandong Vocational School. Sports World (Academic Edition), No. 5, 133-134+136.
[ ] Fu, F., Wang, G., Hu, Y., et al. (2018) Innovative Research on the Teaching Methods of Line Dance Course in Ordinary Colleges and Universities. Journal of Guangzhou Sport University, 38, 131-134.
[ ] Liu, S. (2017) A Comparative Study of Advantages and Disadvantages of Line Dance, Aerobics and Sports Dance. Intelligence, No. 23, 236-236.
[ ] Tang, X. (2018) Exploration and Practice of Promotion of “Line Dance” in Higher Vocational Colleges-Take Hunan Electrical College of Technology as an Example. Modernization of Education, 5, 334-335+365.
[ ] Yang, S., Jiang, Y., Song, D., et al. (2018) Research on “Popular Line Dance Service Gang” under the Background of College Students’ Innovation and Entrepreneurship. Digital Communication World, 166, 282.
[ ] Chen, J. (2017) Discussion about the Present Situation and Development Strategy of Line Dance Teaching in Colleges and Universities. Contemporary Sports Science and Technology, 7, 101-102.
  •   Articles
  •   Archive
  •   Indexing
  •   Aims & Scope
  •   Editorial Board
  •   For Authors
  •   Publication Fees

Journals Menu  

  • Open Special Issues
  • Published Special Issues
  • Special Issues Guideline
  • E-Mail Alert
  • OALibJ Subscription
  • Publication Ethics & OA Statement
  • Frequently Asked Questions
  • Recommend to Peers
  • Recommend to Library
  • History Issue
+1 323-425-8868
+86 18163351462(WhatsApp)
Paper Publishing WeChat

Copyright © 2024 by authors and Scientific Research Publishing Inc.

Creative Commons License

This work and the related PDF file are licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License .

  • Journals A-Z

About SCIRP

  • Publication Fees
  • For Authors
  • Peer-Review Issues
  • Special Issues
  • Manuscript Tracking System
  • Subscription
  • Translation & Proofreading
  • Volume & Issue
  • Open Access
  • Publication Ethics
  • Preservation
  • Privacy Policy

The best way to conference proceedings by Francis Academic Press

Web of Proceedings - Francis Academic Press

  • PROCEEDINGS SERIES
  • FORTHCOMING CONFERENCES
  • FOR ORGANIZERS
  • FOR AUTHORS

Web of Proceedings - Francis Academic Press

  • Proceedings Series
  • ICHCAD 2020

An Empirical Study on Line Dancing to Promote the Construction of Community Sports Culture

line dance research paper

DOI: 10.25236/ICHCAD.2020.105

Corresponding Author

U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings

Preview improvements coming to the PMC website in October 2024. Learn More or Try it out now .

  • Advanced Search
  • Journal List
  • Front Psychol

Effects of Dance Movement Therapy and Dance on Health-Related Psychological Outcomes. A Meta-Analysis Update

Sabine c. koch.

1 Department of Creative Arts Therapies and Therapy Sciences, Alanus University, Alfter, Germany

2 School of Therapy Sciences, SRH University Heidelberg, Heidelberg, Germany

Roxana F. F. Riege

3 Department of Psychology, Friedrich-Schiller-University, Jena, Germany

Katharina Tisborn

4 Department of Psychology, University of Bochum, Bochum, Germany

Jacelyn Biondo

5 Department of Creative Arts Therapies, Drexel University, Philadelphia, PA, United States

Lily Martin

Andreas beelmann, associated data.

Background: Dance is an embodied activity and, when applied therapeutically, can have several specific and unspecific health benefits. In this meta-analysis, we evaluated the effectiveness of dance movement therapy 1 (DMT) and dance interventions for psychological health outcomes. Research in this area grew considerably from 1.3 detected studies/year in 1996–2012 to 6.8 detected studies/year in 2012–2018.

Method: We synthesized 41 controlled intervention studies ( N = 2,374; from 01/2012 to 03/2018), 21 from DMT, and 20 from dance, investigating the outcome clusters of quality of life, clinical outcomes (with sub-analyses of depression and anxiety), interpersonal skills, cognitive skills, and (psycho-)motor skills. We included recent randomized controlled trials (RCTs) in areas such as depression, anxiety, schizophrenia, autism, elderly patients, oncology, neurology, chronic heart failure, and cardiovascular disease, including follow-up data in eight studies.

Results: Analyses yielded a medium overall effect ( d 2 = 0.60), with high heterogeneity of results ( I 2 = 72.62%). Sorted by outcome clusters, the effects were medium to large ( d = 0.53 to d = 0.85). All effects, except the one for (psycho-)motor skills, showed high inconsistency of results. Sensitivity analyses revealed that type of intervention (DMT or dance) was a significant moderator of results. In the DMT cluster , the overall medium effect was small, significant, and homogeneous/consistent ( d = 0.30, p < 0.001, I 2 = 3.47). In the dance intervention cluster , the overall medium effect was large, significant, yet heterogeneous/non-consistent ( d = 0.81, p < 0.001, I 2 = 77.96). Results suggest that DMT decreases depression and anxiety and increases quality of life and interpersonal and cognitive skills, whereas dance interventions increase (psycho-)motor skills. Larger effect sizes resulted from observational measures, possibly indicating bias. Follow-up data showed that on 22 weeks after the intervention, most effects remained stable or slightly increased.

Discussion: Consistent effects of DMT coincide with findings from former meta-analyses. Most dance intervention studies came from preventive contexts and most DMT studies came from institutional healthcare contexts with more severely impaired clinical patients, where we found smaller effects, yet with higher clinical relevance. Methodological shortcomings of many included studies and heterogeneity of outcome measures limit results. Initial findings on long-term effects are promising.

Introduction

Why this meta-analysis.

Dance movement therapy (DMT) is the psychotherapeutic use of movement, based on the assumption of the interconnection of body and mind, and the healing power of dance. The American Dance Therapy Association (ADTA) defines it as “the psychotherapeutic use of movement to promote emotional, social, cognitive, and physical integration of the individual, for the purpose of improving health and well-being” (ADTA, 2018 ); the European Association Dance Movement Therapy adds the “spiritual integration” to this list (EADMT, 2018 ). At the present state of professionalization, next to the development of knowledge on mechanisms of DMT (Koch, 2017 ), and of arts-based research methods (Hervey, 2000 ; Leavy, 2017 ), the implementation of evidence-based research is crucial for the survival, recognition, and thriving of the clinical field of DMT (see, e.g., Wengrower and Chaiklin, 2008 ; Bräuninger, 2012a , b ; Dunphy et al., 2019 ) in the worldwide healthcare systems.

While the use of dance as a healing art is presumably as old as mankind, DMT became an established profession in Western countries from the 1940s, when the first pioneers developed professional dance therapy concepts, which spread in Eastern countries and worldwide beginning in the 1990s. Most DMT professionals work in psychiatric hospitals, rehabilitation centers, schools, and private practice. Throughout its existence, there has been much qualitative research in DMT (see, e.g., Goodill, 2005 ; Cruz and Berrol, 2012 ). However, in recent years, evidence-based quantitative research is getting stronger, in terms of numbers and quality of studies (e.g., Peters, 2012 ). A more general empirical evidence-base on the effectiveness of DMT will provide an orientation for patients, therapists, researchers, educators, and healthcare associations and influence professional and public recognition of DMT (Koch et al., 2014 ). According to standards of evidence-based medical practice, there are five levels of evidence (from the bottom to the top level): Level 4: expert opinions/textbooks, Level 3: case studies/non-experimental studies, Level 2: quasi-experimental studies, Level 1b: randomized controlled studies, and Level 1a: meta-analyses/reviews (e.g., Sackett et al., 2000 ). The present meta-analysis is an update of the meta-analysis published by Koch et al. ( 2014 ), tackling high-quality evidence-based research in the field. The aim was to synthesize data from controlled intervention studies published worldwide between January of 2012 and March of 2018.

Since dance is an important therapeutic ingredient in DMT, we also included studies on the effect of dance interventions , similar to the previous meta-analysis (Koch et al., 2014 ). There is a growing number of artists offering dance interventions in clinical and subclinical contexts (see Martin et al., 2018 ). Including dance intervention studies in addition to DMT studies had the further advantage that we obtained enough data to conduct sensitivity analysis. Dance interventions in this context are practices of various dance styles (e.g., ballroom dance, folk dance, contemporary dance), which aim to improve the quality of life or other health-related psychological outcomes of the participants. It is possible to synthesize dance intervention and DMT intervention studies, because they presumably share many therapeutic mechanisms (also termed active factors , denominating the effective ingredient of an intervention; Kazdin, 2007 ; Koch, 2017 ). Koch ( 2017 ) has distinguished five mechanism clusters through which creative arts therapies 3 work that also apply to DMT and dance: (a) hedonism (pleasure and play, non-goal orientation), (b) aesthetic experience (experiencing beauty, body–mind unity, unity with a partner, etc.) and its authentic expression, (c) non-verbal meaning-making (communication, emotion expression and regulation, social interaction), (d) enactive transitional space (experiencing activity, agency, self-efficacy, constructive resources, test-acting, enactment, rituals, and transformation), and (e) creation (generativity, productivity; see Koch, 2017 ). In addition, there are mechanisms such as movement per se (arousal, hormonal changes, physiological changes through movement), dance per se (Jola and Calmeiro, 2017 ), as well as specific body feedback mechanisms related to distinct movement shape changes and qualities (Koch et al., 2007 , 2014 ). Moreover, there are mechanisms of health-related changes that DMT shares with other forms of psychotherapy such as the therapeutic relationship, problem actualization, resource activation, etc. (Grawe et al., 1994 ; Wampold, 2015 ; Wampold and Irmel, 2015 ). Other more specific therapeutic mechanisms are connected to techniques of DMT, such as mirroring, movement analysis, non-verbal metaphors, imaginative techniques, meditative techniques, introspection, and focusing (Bräuninger, 2014 ). Furthermore, there are also specific group mechanisms of change. For example, if the intervention is conducted in a group setting, such mechanisms as cohesion, experience to be part of something larger, mutual trust, corrective emotional experiences, empowerment, mutual support, probing social roles, and enactive interpersonal learning are important (Schmais, 1985 , 1998 ; Yalom, 1985 ; Rutan and Stone, 2001 ). More research is needed to deepen and sharpen the knowledge on therapeutic mechanism of DMT and dance interventions, and in turn improve outcome research in both fields.

The guiding questions and aim of this meta-analysis are to shed light on (a) the extent in which DMT and dance interventions initiate desired changes in health-related psychological outcomes, (b) the moderators or study characteristics that contribute to the variation of the effect sizes, and (c) the longevity or duration of these changes.

State of Research

In this section, we will provide an overview on the secondary trials (meta-analyses/systematic reviews) and some general information on the primary trials conducted on effects of DMT and dance between January 2012 and March 2018.

Overview of Meta-Analyses and Reviews (Secondary Trials)

Four general meta-analyses up to 2014.

The first general meta-analysis on DMT by Ritter and Graff Low ( 1996 ) provided a broad overview on the effects of DMT on health-related outcomes, incorporating 23 primary studies up to the year 1995. It yielded promising results about the effectiveness of DMT across various populations and diagnoses (children; adults; elderly; non-clinical, subclinical, and clinical populations; physical and psychiatric disorders), particularly improved anxiety symptoms. Furthermore, the authors reported health-improving changes in psychological conditions, movement, body-awareness, anger, and self-concept. Nevertheless, the study by Ritter and Graff Low ( 1996 ) had some limitations. The methodological quality of the incorporated studies varied considerably, and the authors did not report any analyses of heterogeneity (see critique of Cruz and Sabers, 1998 ).

Koch et al. ( 2014 ) replicated most of these findings. In their general meta-analysis on the effectiveness of DMT and dance interventions, they incorporated 23 primary studies published between 1995 and 2012. Results suggested that DMT and dance interventions improve anxiety levels [post-value comparison of standard mean differences (SMD; for a list of statistics and symbol abbreviation, see Appendix A ), taking into account the confidence interval (CI): SMD = 0.44, CI = 0.15–0.72], depression (SMD = 0.36, CI = 0.17–0.56), well-being (SMD = 0.30, CI = 0.07–0.53), quality of life (SMD = 0.37, CI = 0.18–0.55), and body image (SMD = 0.27, CI = –0.04–0.57). They also found a positive effect for interpersonal competence (SMD = 0.45, CI = 0.07–0.83), but this effect was inconsistent across studies ( I 2 = 52%). The results of this study ought to be interpreted with caution due to several methodological constraints of the primary trials, small analysis clusters (four to eight studies per outcome cluster), and broad confidence intervals.

In sum, we found 11 meta-analyses and nine reviews on effects of dance and DMT, published after the last literature search in 2012 of Koch et al. ( 2014 ) up to March 2018. Only one of these publications was a more general overview on the effects of DMT on depression, anxiety, and well-being. It was a Master thesis by Peters ( 2012 ), incorporating 26 randomized or quasi-randomized trials. The results indicated that dance/DMT had small, but significant and positive effects on general well-being ( d = 0.29, I 2 = 38.65%), depression ( d = 0.33, I 2 = 42.04%), and anxiety ( d = 0.31, I 2 = 2.97), corroborating with the findings of Koch et al. ( 2014 ). The effects for general well-being and depression were moderated by type of therapist (with specialized dance instructors and DMT therapists yielding larger effects than non-specialized therapists, physiotherapists, exercise instructors, or researchers), but were not moderated by age, gender, type of dance , or type of country .

Eighteen specific (disease-related) reviews

The remaining 18 reviews and meta-analyses on effects of DMT and dance interventions focused on patient samples with specific diagnoses: seven on Parkinson's disease, four on anxiety and depression, four on physiological impairments, three on dementia/elderly, and two on oncology. The reviews on Parkinson's disease investigated the effects of DMT and dance on motor function and quality of life (De Dreu et al., 2012 ; Kiepe et al., 2012 ; Hackney and Bennett, 2014 ; Sharp and Hewitt, 2014 ; Loetzke et al., 2015 ; Shanahan et al., 2015 ; Aguiar et al., 2016 ). Significant improvements in balance (Berg Balance Scale), motor impairments (UPDRS-Scale, Timed-Up-and-Go scale), and quality of life were found in the intervention groups. Some of the effects remained significant, when comparing the results to a control group. We also found studies that yielded no improvements in quality of life and motor function (e.g., freezing of gait). Most trials reported participants' satisfaction and high adherence to the treatment. The secondary trials on anxiety and depression reported effects of DMT and dance on both outcomes including psychological distress (Kiepe et al., 2012 ; Peters, 2012 ; Boehm et al., 2014 ; Meekums et al., 2015 ). A high-quality primary trial was the study by Jeong et al. ( 2005 ) on health improvements through DMT in adolescent girls with mild depression. It showed increasing plasma serotonin concentration and decreasing concentration of dopamine in the participants of the DMT intervention group compared to participants of the control group. The effects on medical conditions such as heart failure and hypertension were investigated by Conceição et al. ( 2016 ), Gomes Neto et al. ( 2014 ), Kiepe et al. ( 2012 ), and Rodrigues-Krause et al. ( 2016 ). The results indicate that DMT (in this context termed “medical DMT”; Goodill, 2005 ) and dance might improve physiological conditions (e.g., systolic and diastolic blood pressure, V02-peak, exercise capacity) and quality of life, but not beyond the effects of other exercise interventions. One review on dementia and DMT in care homes showed that problematic behaviors decreased and social interaction and enjoyment in residents and care staff increased; adverse effects were also acknowledged, for example, from the care staff's perspective, there were fears of over-attachment with residents or embarrassment with dancing and concerns about staff shortages affecting the organization of regular dance sessions in the home (Guzmán-García et al., 2013 ). Some residents showed signs of confusion, irritability, and anxiety during the dancing (Palo-Bengtsson and Ekman, 1997 ; Palo-Bengtsson et al., 1998 ). The meta-analysis on DMT and dementia by Karkou and Meekums ( 2017 ) did not include any studies, because no trials met the inclusion criteria (i.e., randomized controlled trial led by dance movement therapist). Regarding the effects of DMT and dance on quality of life in cancer patients , we found mixed results. One meta-analysis on DMT in oncology reported significant standard mean differences (SMD) for the reduction of anxiety (Boehm et al., 2014 ; see also Archer et al., 2015 ). The meta-analysis on DMT in oncology reports effects on quality of life, but no evidence for effects on depression, anxiety, stress, fatigue, or body image (Bradt et al., 2011 , 2015 ).

Because of the scarcity or low quality of evidence-based research in the field of DMT, most secondary studies (particularly the high quality Cochrane reviews) included only a few studies (between one and three studies). Besides that, most authors reported that their results did not have sufficient statistical power, because of methodological constraints of the primary trials. Nevertheless, in previous quantitative research, we can observe a pattern that DMT and dance interventions are as effective as traditional psychotherapy in various populations and conditions (Beelmann and Heinrichs, 2015 ). In addition, some high-quality qualitative research suggests that DMT and dance are beneficial supportive treatments to traditional care and have some specific advantages, such as the non-verbal approach, body-oriented treatment process, etc. (McNiff, 1993 ; Barba et al., 1995 ; Hervey, 2000 ; Levine and Land, 2016 ).

Information on Primary Trials

Included studies.

All included trials are displayed in Table 1 in the results section and are marked with an “ * ” in the reference section. They were at least controlled intervention studies. We distinguished between primary studies in DMT and primary studies in dance interventions, and found 21 studies on the effects of DMT and 20 studies on the effects of dance on health-related outcomes (see Table 1 for populations, criteria, and results).

Study characteristics chart of the included studies.

)
1Dynamic neuro-cognitive imagery improves mental imagery ability, disease severity, and motor and cognitive functions in people with Parkinson's diseaseAbraham et al., USA
NEG = 10
NCG = 10
DMT, Active (Learning
+ Exercise)
Parkinson disease,
Elderly (MEG = 66.4; SDEG = 12.5)
2 weeks
5 × per week
120 min
Yes,
Not Reported
0.41
2Effectiveness of a combined dance and relaxation intervention on reducing anxiety and depression and improving quality of life among the cognitively impaired elderlyAdam et al., Malaysia
NEG = 44
NCG = 40
Dance (poco-poco dance), Active (Relaxation exercise)Cognitive deficits,
Elderly (M = 70.87;
SD = 8.19)
6 weeks
2 × per week
60 min
No,
Not Reported
1.43
3Backing the backbones—a feasibility study of effectiveness of dance movement psychotherapy on parenting stress in caregivers of children with autism spectrumAithal and Karkou, India
NEG = 5
NCG = 6
DMT (Indian techniques: nritta, nrutya, natya), Non-ActiveCaregivers of children with ASD,
Adults (28–35 years)
2 weeks
3 × per week
Minutes not reported
No,
8.33%
1.80
4Dance therapy combined with patient education improves quality of life of persons with obesity: a pilot feasibility study for a randomized controlled trialAllet et al., Switzerland
NEG = 34
NCG = 33
DMT, Active (Educational sessions)Obesity,
Adults (M = 46.19; SD = 10.15)
16 weeks
2 × per week
60 min
Quasi,
20.37%
0.38
5Effects of dance movement therapy on selected cardiovascular parameters and estimated maximum oxygen consumption in hypertensive patientsAweto et al., Nigeria
NEG = 23
NCG = 15
DMT, Active (Educational sessions)Hypertension,
Adults (MEG = 44.1; SDEG = 12.7)
4 weeks
2 × per week
50 min
Yes,
24%
0.50
6Effectiveness of dance in patients with fibromyalgia: a randomized, single-blind, controlled studyBaptista et al., Brazil
NEG = 38
NCG = 37
Dance (Belly dance), Non-activeFybromyalgia,
Adults (18–65 years)
16 weeks
1 × per week
60 min
Yes,
6.66%
0.48
7The effects of folk dance training on 5–6 years children's physical and social developmentBiber, Turkey
NEG = 20
NCG = 20
Dance (Folk Dance), Non-active (TAU)No Diagnosis,
Children (5–6 years)
8 weeks
4 × per week
40 min
No,
0%
2.32
8Dance movement therapy group intervention in stress treatment: a randomized controlled trial (RCT) AND The efficacy of dance movement therapy group on improvement of quality of life: a randomized controlled trialBräuninger, ,bGermany
NEG = 97
NCG = 65
DMT, Non-activeStressed,
Adults (16–65 years)
12 weeks
1 × per week
90 min
Yes,
8.02%
0.36
9Enhancing positive affect and divergent thinking abilities: Play some music and danceCampion and Levita, UK
NEG = 15
NCG1 = 13
NCG2 = 14
NCG3 = 14
Dance, active (Quiet, listening to music, cycling)No diagnosis
Young adults (18–23 years)
One session
5 min
Yes,
7.14%
−0.17
10Efficacy of caregiver-mediated joint engagement intervention for young children with autism spectrum disordersChiang et al., Taiwan
NEG = 18
NCG = 16
DMT (Creative movement play) + adapted joint engagement, Non-active (TAU)Autism
Children (2–4 years)
8 weeks
2 × per week
60 min
Quasi,
Not Reported
0.61
11Creative dance improves physical fitness and life satisfaction in older womenCruz-Ferreira et al., Portugal
NEG = 32
NCG = 25
Dance (Creative dance), Non-activeNo diagnosis elderly (65–80 years)24 weeks
3 × per Week
50 min
Yes,
0%
2.54
12Dance therapy improves motor and cognitive functions in patients with Parkinson's diseaseDe Natale et al., Italy
NEG = 9
NCG = 7
Dance (Tango), Non-active (TAU)Parkinson disease
Elderly (M = 67; SD = 6.9)
10 weeks
2 × per week
60 min
No,
12.5%
0.25
13Influencing self-rated health among adolescent girls with dance interventionDuberg et al., Sweden
NEG = 48
NCG = 53
Dance (African, Jazz, Contemporary), Non-activeNo diagnosis
Teenager (13–18 years)
32 weeks
2 × per week
75 min
Yes,
Not Reported
0.51
14Cognitive benefits of a dance movement therapy program in adults with intellectual disabilitiesGuerra-Balic et al., Spain
NEG = 13
NCG = 15
DMT (Chacian approach), Non-activeIntellectual disability
Adults (44–66 years)
13 weeks
2 × per week
60 min
No,
Not reported
0.11
15Effects of dance on motor functions, cognitive functions, and mental symptoms of Parkinson's disease: a quasi-randomized pilot trialHashimoto et al., Japan
NEG = 15
NCG = 14
Dance (Modern, Jazz, Ballet, Aerobic), Active (physical exercise) + Non-activeParkinson-disease
Elderly (Mean = 67.9; SD = 7)
12 weeks
1 × per week
60 min
Quasi,
21.74%
0.71
16“We dance and find each other”: Effects of dance/movement therapy on negative symptoms in autism spectrum disorderHildebrandt et al., Germany
NEG = 53
NCG = 22
DMT, Non-activeASD
Teenager + Adults (14–53 years)
10 weeks
1 × per week
60 min
Yes,
57.38%
0.24
17Effects of a short-term dance movement therapy program on symptoms and stress in patients with breast cancer undergoing radiotherapy: a randomized, controlled, single-blind trialHo et al., Hong Kong
NEG = 69
NCG = 70
DMT, Non-active (TAU)Breast cancer
Adults (18+years)
3 weeks
2 × per week
90 min
Yes,
10.77%
0.06
18Effects of exercise training with traditional dancing on functional capacity and quality of life in patients with schizophrenia: a randomized controlled studyKaltsatou et al., Greece
NEG = 16
NCG = 15
Dance (Greek traditional dance), Non-active (TAU, e.g., Psycho-therapy)Schizophrenia
Adults (Mean = 59.9; SD = 14.1)
32 weeks
3 × per week
60 min
Yes,
0%
0.35
19Six months of dance intervention enhances postural, sensorimotor, and cognitive performance in elderly without affecting cardio-respiratory functionsKattenstroth et al., Germany
NEG = 25
NCG = 10
Dance (“Agilando”-special dance for elderly), Non-activeNo diagnosis
Elderly (60–94 years)
24 weeks
1 × per week
60 min
Quasi,
Not Reported
0.65
20Fixing the mirrors a feasibility study of dance movement therapy on young adults with autism spectrum disorderKoch et al., Germany
NEG = 16
NCG = 15
DMT, Non-activeAutism
Young adults (16–47 years)
7 weeks
1 × per week
60 min
Quasi,
0%
0.55
21Breaking barriers: evaluating an arts-based emotion regulation training in prisonKoch et al., Germany
NEG = 29
NCG = 18
DMT based anti-violence training, Non-activeNo diagnosis, Imprisoned
Adults (21–63)
1 week
5 × per week
60 min
Quasi,
Not Reported
0.02
22Embodied self in trauma and self-harm: effects of a single flamenco therapy session on traumatized in-patients. A pilot studyKoch et al., Germany
NEG = 16
NCG = 16
DMT (Flamenco based), Non-active (TAU)Trauma
Adults (18–59 years)
1 week
1 × per week
60 min
Quasi,
2.13%
0.54
23Fostering social cognition through an imitation
and synchronization-based dance-movement intervention in adults with autism spectrum disorder: a controlled proof-of-concept study
Koehne et al., Germany
NEG = 27
NCG = 24
Dance (Synchronization based movement intervention), Active (Controlled movement intervention)High-function ASD
Young adults (M = 32.75; SD = 9.1)
10 weeks
1 × per week
90 min
No,
7.27%
0.34
24Effectiveness of dance/movement therapy on affect and psychotic symptoms in patients with schizophreniaLee et al., Korea
NEG = 18
NCG = 20
DMT, Non-active (TAU)Schizophrenia
Adults (M = 41.5; SD = 9.1)
12 weeks
1 × per week
60 min
Quasi,
Not Reported
0.28
25Overcoming disembodiment: The effect of movement therapy on negative symptoms in schizophrenia—A multicenter randomized controlled trialMartin et al., Germany
NEG = 44
NCG = 24
DMT, Non-active (TAU)Schizophrenia
Adults (M = 39.84; SD = 10.35)
10 weeks
2 × per week
90 min
Yes,
30.88%
0.74
26Does 12-week Latin dance training affect the self-confidence of the university students?Meric and Ilhan, Turkey
NEG = 30
NCG = 30
Dance (Latin), Not describedNo diagnosis
Young adults (M = 20.4; SD = 1.99)
12 weeks
1 × per week
120 min
No,
Not reported
0.66
27Dance therapy and the public school: The development of social and emotional skills of high school students in GreecePanagiotopoulou, Greece
NEG = 11
NCG = 12
DMT, Non-activeNo diagnosis
Teenager (16–17 years)
12 weeks
1 × per week
60 min
No,
0%
0.29
28Argentine tango dance compared to mindfulness meditation and a waiting-list control: A randomized trial for treating depressionPinniger et al., Australia
NEG = 21
NCG1 = 29
NCG2 = 16
Dance (Argentinean Tango), Active (physical exercise) + Non-activeStress
Adults (18–80 years)
24 weeks
1 × per week
90 min
Yes,
31.96%
0.39
29Tango dance can reduce distress and insomnia in people with self-referred affective symptomsPinniger et al., Australia
NEG = 24
NCG1 = 25
NCG2 = 11
NCG3 = 12
Dance Argentinean Tango Active (Meditation, Exercise), Non-activeStress
Adults (18–68)
8 weeks
1 × per week
90 min
Yes,
34.02%
0.67
30Changes in well-being of schizophrenic patients after movement therapy: results of a multicenter RCT-studyPohlmann et al., Germany
NEG = 24
NCG = 12
DMT, Non-activeSchizophrenia
Adults (18–83)
20 weeks
1 × per week
90 min
Yes,
47.06%
0.50
31Effectiveness of group body psychotherapy for negative symptoms of schizophrenia: multicentre randomized controlled trialPriebe et al., UK
NEG = 140
NCG = 135
DMT (Manualised body psychotherapy), Active (Pilates)Schizophrenia
Adults (M = 42.2; SD = 10.7)
10 weeks
2 × per week
90 min
Yes,
4.54%
0.19
32A dance movement therapy group for depressed adult patients in a psychiatric outpatient clinic: Effects of the treatmentPylvänäinen et al., Finland
NEG = 21
NCG = 12
DMT (Chacian approach), Non-active (TAU)Depression
Adults (20–59 years)
12 weeks
1 × per week
90 min
No,
22%
0.74
33Tango for treatment of motor and non-motor manifestations in Parkinson's disease: A randomized control studyRios Romenets et al., Canada
NEG = 18
NCG = 15
Dance (Argentinean Tango), Active (Self-directed exercise)Parkinson-disease
Elderly MEG = 63.2; SDEG = 9.9)
12 weeks
2 × per week
60 min
Yes,
27.27%
0.20
34An exploratory randomized controlled trial of
body psychotherapy for patients with chronic depression
Röhricht et al., UK
NEG = 16
NCG = 15
DMT, Non-Active (TAU)Depression
Adults (18–65 years)
10 weeks
2 × per week
90 min
Yes,
24.24%
1.16
35Effect of dance on cancer-related fatigue and quality of lifeSerrano-Guzmán et al., Spain
NEG = 35
NCG = 32
DMT, Non-Active (TAU)Hypertension
Adults + Elderly (62–76)
8 weeks
3 × per week
50 min
Yes,
0%
0.55
36Effect of dance on cancer-related fatigue and quality of lifeSturm et al., Germany
NEG = 20
NCG = 20
Dance (Group choreography), Non-activeCancer
Adults (26–74)
5 weeks
2 × per week
60 min
Quasi,
10%
1.03
37Dance improves functionality and psychosocial adjustment in cerebral palsyTeixeira-Machado et al., Brazil
NEG = 13
NCG = 13
Dance, active (kinesiotherapy)Cerebral Palsy
Teenager + Young adults (15–29 years)
12 weeks
2 × per week
60 min
Yes,
0%
2.79
38The effect of dance on depressive symptoms in nursing home residentsVankova et al., Czech Republic162
NEG = 79
NCG = 83
Dance (Exercise dance for seniors), Non-active (TAU)Various diagnosis
Elderly in nursing homes (60+ years)
12 weeks
1 × per week
60 min
Yes,
Not reported
0.32
39A pilot study to evaluate multi-dimensional effects of dance for people with Parkinson's diseaseVentura et al., USA
NEG = 8
NCG = 7
Dance (Ballet, Musical-Jazz) + DMT Elements, active (Parkinson support group)Parkinson Disease
Elderly (MEG = 71.8; SDEG = 3.6)
10 weeks
1 × per week
75 min
No,
0%
0.61
40A comparison of Irish set dancing and exercises for people with Parkinson's disease: a phase II feasibility studyVolpe et al., Italy
NEG = 12
NCG = 12
Dance (Irish set dancing), Active (Standard physiotherapy exercises)Parkinson- disease
Elderly
(56–73 years)
24 weeks
1 × per week
90 min
Yes,
0%
0.99
41Active factors in dance/movement therapy: Specifying health effects of non-goal-orientation
in movement
Wiedenhofer and Koch, Germany
NEG = 28
NCG = 28
DMT (Improvisational non-goal-oriented movements)No diagnosis
Adults (19–49 years)
One session
50 min
Quasi,
1.79%
0.25

NEG, number of subjects in the intervention group; NCG, number of subjects in the control group; M, mean; MEG, mean in intervention group; SDEG, standard deviation in intervention group; TAU, treatment as usual; ES, effect size. Bold values indicate the total sample of each study .

Excluded studies

We located a number of high-quality primary studies that we were not able to include due to the defined criteria. Fourteen studies had been excluded due to missing data or other reasons outlined in the Methods section, some of which may be possibly recovered for future analyses. They investigated learning disabilities (Alotaibi et al., 2017 ), children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (Alrazain et al., 2018 ), fornix integrity (Burzynska et al., 2017 ), depression (Cross et al., 2012 ), falls (Duim et al., 2015 ), oncological patients (Ho et al., 2016b ), schizophrenia (Koch et al., 2017 ; Savill et al., 2017 ), Parkinson's disease (Lewis et al., 2014 ), autism (Mateos-Moreno and Atencia-Doña, 2013 ), trauma in unaccompanied minors (Meyer DeMott et al., 2017 ), development of kindergarteners (Stück and Villegas, 2017 ), personal development and increase of emotional intelligence in students (Vancea, 2013 ), and traumatized children (Van Westrhenen et al., 2019 ).

Mainly through the reviews, we also found a number of formerly undetected studies from 2011 and earlier, not yet included into the general meta-analyses. We consider it important to enumerate them here for potential future analyses: Belardinelli et al. ( 2008 ), Burgess et al. ( 2006 ), Chouhan and Kumar ( 2011 ), Connolly et al. ( 2011 ), Coubard et al. ( 2011 ), Hall ( 2011 ), Hwang et al. ( 2010 ), Kaltsatou et al. ( 2011 , 2015 ), Quiroga Murcia et al. ( 2009 ), and Xiong and Li ( 2009 ).

In general, our literature search revealed that there were at least as many studies on physiological changes after DMT and dance interventions as there were for psychological changes in health outcomes in the time frame of January 2012 to March 2018. We found around 50 primary studies with mere physical/physiological outcomes, which we excluded. However, since embodiment approaches such as DMT assume the body–mind unity, we would encourage researchers to include studies focusing on physical changes into future meta-analyses in dance and DMT.

Incorporated Outcomes

Synthesizing the outcome foci of the primary studies, this meta-analysis differentiates six outcome clusters: (a) quality of life, (b) clinical outcomes (e.g., anxiety, depression), (c) interpersonal skills, (d) cognitive skills, (e) (psycho-)motor skills, and (f) residuals (psychotic symptoms and physiological change). We based the allocation of dependent variables to outcome clusters on the meta-analysis of Koch et al. ( 2014 ) and detailed investigation of the primary trials. Our aim was to synthesize outcome clusters that were as comparable as possible (e.g., by similarity of measurement instruments).

Quality of Life

Quality of life is a broad construct, which contains subscales about subjective well-being (e.g., satisfaction with life) and conditions of daily living (e.g., general health, functional capacity and social integration). We decided to further include dependent variables about sleep quality, pain (invert coding), self-esteem, and control beliefs in this cluster. Most measures in this outcome cluster were based on self-report questionnaires (e.g., rating satisfaction with certain life conditions on a Likert scale), with only one study containing observations and ratings from an external person, who in this case was a clinician (Teixeira-Machado et al., 2017 ). We assume that DMT and dance interventions might influence quality of life on various dimensions, for example, movement might improve vitality and fitness, dance might foster joyful experiences (Koch et al., 2007 ), and interpersonal experiences might have a positive influence on social integration (Sandel et al., 1993 ).

Clinical Outcomes

Clinical outcomes summarize dependent variables directly related to conditions of mental health, particularly affective disorders (e.g., depression, anxiety, stress, anger). Studies in this cluster were conducted with a clinical (e.g., persons with a diagnosis of depression) or subclinical population (e.g., persons at risk for depression). Usually, the dependent variables were assessed with self-report questionnaires (e.g., HADS, BDI, BSI, STAXI). Two studies contained an interview and one study used external observation and rating. We decided to conduct a sub-analysis of anxiety and depression, because the treatment of these conditions is of broad public interest and has been discussed in the previous literature (Peters, 2012 ; Koch et al., 2014 ). We assumed that DMT and dance interventions improve psychological functions of emotion regulation, which may be mediated, for example, by authentic expression, experienced agency, body–mind integration, and physiological changes.

Interpersonal Skills

The term “interpersonal skills” relates to competences persons apply in social interaction (e.g., empathy, synchronization, communication, prosocial behavior, self-other awareness, maintaining a relationship). Most studies in this cluster were conducted with children, some of whom had been diagnosed with developmental disorders (ASD, ADHD). Therefore, researchers used external observations (e.g., by a parent, teacher, or clinician) more frequently. There were also two adult populations (ASD, schizophrenia). Interpersonal experiences in DMT and dance might particularly improve skills allocated to this cluster, for example, the therapeutic relationship, group cohesion, and (non-verbal) communication. In recent years, researchers have started to investigate whether the “mirroring” technique, proposed by dance movement therapist Marian Chace (Sandel et al., 1993 ), fosters empathy and enhances activity of mirror neurons in the brain (McGarry and Russo, 2011 ). A link of mirroring in movement and attachment has recently been established (see Feniger-Schaal et al., 2018 , this issue).

Cognitive Skills

Cognitive skills relate to the set of mental abilities and processes that we need to carry out any task from the simplest to the most complex (e.g., skills of language, memory, and conceptualizing). Because a decrease of these mental abilities is a typical issue in elderly persons, most primary studies focus on this population. Cognitive skills were assessed using psychological tests (e.g., memory tasks, word tasks, calculation tasks, attention tasks) and tests referring to body image or body imagery, which denominates the ability to perceive and visualize bodily charges and changes. Since the operationalizations of the concept in the included studies measure a mainly representational skill, we categorize it under cognitive skills, even though it includes sensorimotor and emotional aspects.

(Psycho-)Motor Skills

The assessment of (psycho-)motor skills was conducted in (mostly elderly) patients that were diagnosed with Parkinson's disease. It contained tests on walking, turning, balance, and freezing of gait, and self-report measures on daily functioning. Only dance interventions, no DMT, were found in this outcome cluster. Dance improves motor function by training muscular activity, balance, and flexibility. Furthermore, there is a training of cognitive skills associated with movement, for example, in executing imagined movements, following music, and observing bodily changes (Hashimoto et al., 2015 ). Because we focused on psychological changes in this meta-analysis, studies with outcomes on mere physical skills (e.g., exercise capacity, arm range) were excluded. However, in our literature search, we found at least as many studies on physiological changes after DMT and dance interventions as on psychological changes. It is important to note that changes in Parkinson's disease severity were categorized as psychomotor skills , because Parkinson's disease is an extrapyramidal and neurodegenerative disorder (ICD 10), which entails physical and psychological components. The included studies with a focus on Parkinson's disease measured mixed psycho-physiological variables (as outlined above).

Our residual category contained two types of outcomes too small to analyze in separate clusters: positive symptoms in schizophrenia and physiological changes (e.g., blood pressure). In schizophrenia, we distinguish positive symptoms (an overabundance of perceptions compared to average, e.g., hallucinations) from negative symptoms (a void or lack of perception and expression compared to average, e.g., apathy, mood, and blunted affect). While positive symptoms can be successfully addressed by anti-psychotic medication, DMT seems to be particularly useful for addressing negative symptoms (Röhricht and Priebe, 2006 ; Lee et al., 2015 ; Martin et al., 2016 ; Pohlmann et al., 2017 ). We allocated the negative symptoms to the clinical outcomes cluster. Pohlmann et al. ( 2017 ) postulated a concept of “disembodiment” in schizophrenia stressing that schizophrenia is a self-disorder and is characterized by disturbances of ipseity (selfhood). They state that mechanisms of body–mind integration improve self-awareness. Regarding physiological changes, the effects of dance and DMT are also detectable on a neural and hormonal level (e.g., Quiroga Murcia et al., 2009 ; Stück and Villegas, 2017 ; Abraham et al., 2018 ). Studies assessed positive symptoms in schizophrenia with self-report questionnaires or clinical interviews/observations, whereas physiological data were collected using medical examination procedures (e.g., sphygmomanometer).

Study Selection

The following inclusion criteria were used to filter studies for meta-analysis:

  • Experimental intervention study (independent variable: dance or DMT intervention, dependent variable: health-related psychological outcomes)
  • Control group design
  • Availability of necessary statistics to calculate effect sizes (pre- and post-intervention assessment in intervention and control group, mean, SD, N, t , or F values)
  • Language of publication: English or German
  • Period of publication: 01/2012 to 04/2018

Because we wanted to get a broad picture, we also included studies with interventions named “body psychotherapy” or “movement integration.” We decided to classify an intervention as “DMT intervention” if a dance movement therapist conducted the session. When the qualification of therapist was missing (or unclear), we closely analyzed the descriptions of the intervention. If the intervention description suggested that predominantly typical tools of DMT were used (therapeutic use of dance/movement involving mirroring, conscious social interaction in movement, introspection and reflections on movement and body sensations; see also definition of DMT and description of DMT methods in Koch, 2019 ), we categorized the intervention as DMT. Trials that included dance elements but predominantly used methods of other creative arts therapies were excluded (Mateos-Moreno and Atencia-Doña, 2013 ; Jakobsen et al., 2017 ; Van Westrhenen et al., 2019 ). In contrast, we allocated methods to the dance intervention group, if they were dance training sessions conducted by dancers or exercise instructors from various backgrounds (physiotherapists, nurses, fitness instructors). We decided to include group as well as individual therapy sessions.

Literature Search

For this meta-analysis, we used multiple search strategies. First, we systematically searched electronic databases, namely, Psyndex, PsycINFO, ERIC, CENTRAL, and Google Scholar. We used different terms for dance movement therapy (DMT) and dance-related interventions as single keywords and combined search terms, putting together the single keywords and terms related to the study design (see Box 1 ).

Search terms.

• dance movement psychotherapy •dance movement therapy
• dance therapy
• therapeutic movement
• dance-effectiveness
• dance
• dance-therapy
• expressive movement
• expressive dance
• dance movement psychotherapy + controlled trial
• dance movement therapy + controlled trial
• dance therapy + controlled trial
• dance movement psychotherapy + random
• dance movement therapy + random
• dance therapy + random
• dance movement psychotherapy + controlled trial
• dance movement therapy + controlled trial
• dance therapy + controlled trial
• dance movement psychotherapy + random
• dance movement therapy + random
• dance therapy + random
• dance movement psychotherapy + controlled trial
• dance movement therapy + controlled trial
• dance therapy + controlled trial
• dance movement psychotherapy + random
• dance movement therapy + random
• dance therapy + random
• dance movement psychotherapy + controlled trial
• dance movement therapy + controlled trial
• dance therapy + controlled trial
• dance movement psychotherapy + random
• dance movement therapy + random dance therapy + random

The number of hits is reported in the flowchart below ( Figure 1 ). Additionally, we conducted a hand search examining professional journals without widespread indexing, sending requests for unpublished and in process EBM studies to national and international professional listservs, and directly wrote to researchers with a history in EBM research on dance therapy, asking for references we may have potentially missed; we also included references that were mentioned in some of the secondary studies and that slipped by our systematic literature search (we received detailed responses from Dr. Vicky Karkou, Dr. Bonnie Meekums, Dr. Iris Bräuninger, Susanne Bender, Indra Majore-Dusele, and others).

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is fpsyg-10-01806-g0001.jpg

Flowchart of selection procedure. Included studies are displayed in the study characteristics overview ( Table 1 ).

The systematic database search yielded 132 records and was supplemented with 59 studies identified through hand search. Because each included research report contained only one study, we did not have to differentiate between the analysis of research reports and studies. After removing duplicates (i.e., the same studies found in different data sources), a total of 183 studies was screened. Fifty-four of these studies (30%) met the inclusion criteria, and their full-article texts were then assessed for eligibility.

We had to exclude 14 studies for the following reasons (more information in Appendix ):

  • - Seven studies due to insufficient data (e.g., three had no data available; for four, it was not possible to calculate reliable effect sizes from the statistics provided)
  • - Four studies because closer examination of the intervention methods revealed that they did not match the criteria of either DMT or dance intervention (e.g., combined creative arts therapies, observations of dance pieces)
  • - One study contained only qualitative data
  • - One study contained no comparable control group (participants of the control group were caregivers of participants with Parkinson's diagnosis in the experimental group)
  • - For one study, data were reported and included from another publication

Finally, we were able to include a total of 41 studies into the analysis (see Figure 1 ).

Coding Procedure

We coded the study characteristics (see Table 1 ) using the following variables:

  • - Identification: author, title, year
  • - Publication: country, publication status
  • - Sample: sample size, age range, percentage of female participants, clinical vs. non-clinical sample, diagnosis
  • - Intervention: DMT or dance, qualification of implementing person, quality of intervention description (major intervention methods of DMT see Koch, 2019 )
  • - Control Group: number of control groups, type of control group activity
  • - Time: one session vs. process, length of intervention period, frequency of intervention, length of session, period follow-up
  • - Methods: type of measurement, type of analysis, randomization, dropout

Data Synthesis

The analysis was done in SPSS (IBM, Version 25), employing meta-analysis macros by Wilson ( 2005 ). To synthesize data, we decided to use calculations of effect sizes. There were four levels of analysis (see Figure 2 ).

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is fpsyg-10-01806-g0002.jpg

Hierarchy of effect sizes. EG, experimental group; CG, control group; d(c), weighted mean effect size per cluster; d(i), weighted mean effect size per study; d(i,c), weighted mean effect size per cluster within one study.

Most studies were using multiple scales or tests to measure the changes from pre- to post-intervention in one dependent construct (e.g., mental health, general health, and vitality as facets of quality of life). Furthermore, usually more than one dependent construct was observed in one study (e.g., quality of life, affect, and interpersonal skills). Thus, we had to synthesize data on several levels ( Figure 2 ). From Level IV to Level III and from Level III to Level II effect sizes within studies were synthesized. From Level II to Level I effect sizes across studies were synthesized. We started our analysis at the bottom level (Level IV), calculating effect sizes for each dependent variable (e.g., scales, tests). An adjusted formula of Cohen's d was used (Cohen, 1988 ):

This formula incorporates differences between pre- and post-intervention values in the intervention group controlling for changes that occurred in the control group (for abbreviations, see list of symbols in Appendix ). The effect is standardized using the pooled standard deviation, which is formed by the pooled variance of intervention and control group at the pre-intervention level. The natural variance of characteristic values in one sample is assumed to be confounded by the intervention; this is why the pooled variances at pre-time are more precise estimates of variation (Lipsey and Wilson, 2001 ).

Some studies reported median instead of mean and standard error of the mean or interquartile range instead of standard deviation. In these cases, we used the median as a mean and recalculated dispersion measures to approximate the effect sizes (Higgins and Green, 2008 ). In addition, we adjusted the polarization of the scales multiplying with −1, where necessary. Where the polarization of scales or tests remained unreported in the original study, we conducted a literature search to ascertain the direction of the effect. All effects were polarized to ensure that a positive effect size means that the health or resources of the participant improved (e.g., more interpersonal skills, less depression). In total, we calculated 306 effect sizes ranging from d = −0.82 to a maximum of 9.61. Sixteen effect sizes were larger than 3.0 and could be viewed as outlier effect sizes that could have serious impact on the mean effect size and the moderator analyses. Therefore, to avoid an overestimation of the effectiveness (Lipsey and Wilson, 2001 ), we recoded all effect sizes greater than three to d = 3.0, because it seems that those effect sizes are not realistic within intervention studies (Beelmann, personal communication). Next, calculating mean effect sizes, we aggregated the data of multiple dependent variables that measured changes in one dependent construct (outcome cluster). As a result, we obtained 78 effect sizes, with each effect size referring to one dependent construct in one study ( d c,i , Level III). Again, we calculated mean effect size, to synthesize the effect sizes of the dependent constructs into one mean effect size per study ( d i , Level II). Then, we planned to analyze data across studies (Level I) using methods developed by Hedges and Olkin ( 1985 ). Because studies with a greater sample size are generally more conclusive than smaller studies, each study was weighted with:

To get one overall weighted mean effect size, we calculated:

To obtain weighted mean effect sizes for outcome clusters (dependent constructs), we used the same formula replacing the mean effect sizes per study ( d i ) with the mean effect sizes per construct per cluster ( d c,i , Level III). Our analysis did not correct for multiple testing in order not to lose power. Instead, the chosen method is strong in avoiding Type I error. Having conducted 78 tests, it is important to note that four results may have been randomly significant (expecting five randomly significant results out of 100 tests).

Assessment of Heterogeneity

Heterogeneity is an important issue in meta-analysis. It deals with the question of whether all included studies measure the same intervention effect. Usually, if there is a more general research question and there are less strict inclusion criteria, as it is the case in our analysis, we expect that we can observe more differences between studies in content- and method-related characteristics. We analyzed the heterogeneity of the effects using Q -statistics. A significant Q means that the heterogeneity is larger than we would expect from sampling error. In this case, we would need to calculate a random instead of a fixed-effect model (Lipsey and Wilson, 2001 ). In a random-effect model, we use a new inverse variance component resulting in greater confidence intervals of the effect sizes. From Q -statistics, we can calculate I -square, which is an estimator for the inconsistency of the results (Higgins et al., 2003 ). It is interpreted as the percentage of variance of the effect that is caused by heterogeneity (25% = low heterogeneity, 50% = moderate heterogeneity, 75% = high heterogeneity; Cohen, 1988 ).

Analysis of Outliers and Publication Bias

Due to the file-drawer problem (i.e., the problem that studies failing to produce a statistically significant result are less likely to be published than those that do produce a statistically significant result), meta-analyses are in danger of overestimating the effects of an intervention (Rosenthal, 1979 ). We used a funnel plot and trim-and-fill analysis, two methods for assessing publication biases, to explore whether this might be the case in our analysis. We also investigated outliers that might bias the results.

Assessment of Sensitivity

To address issues of heterogeneity and to obtain a better understanding of which study characteristics might influence the assessment of effect sizes, we conducted sensitivity analysis for categorical variables (METAF, Macros from David B. Wilson; Lipsey and Wilson, 2001 ) and for metric variables (METAREG, Macros from David B. Wilson; Lipsey and Wilson, 2001 ). The study characteristics were incorporated as moderators of the effects.

To compare different control group types (e.g., waiting-list control group, physical exercise control groups), we additionally conducted separate analysis of studies with more than one control group, to ensure that all other study characteristics remained constant.

Analysis of Follow-Up Data

To obtain information about long-term effects, we created a separate file to analyze follow-up data. Using the same procedure as described above, we calculated the effects from pre-test to follow-up values.

Study Characteristics

Countries of publication.

The incorporated studies came from 14 different countries. Most studies were conducted in Germany (11 studies, 26.8%). Overall, 25 studies (60.98%) were conducted in Europe. In addition, eight studies (19.51%) were conducted in Asia, three studies (7.32%) in North America, two studies (4.88%) in Australia, two studies in South America (4.88%), and one study in Africa (2.44%). Generally, one can observe that most studies stemmed from “Western” countries (Germany, UK, USA, Canada, Australia, Mediterranean, and Scandinavian countries). Most Asian studies were conducted in Hong Kong, Korea, and Japan. The remaining studies stemmed from Malaysia, India, and Nigeria. We found seven studies that had not been published yet (Aweto et al., 2012 ; Ventura et al., 2016 ; De Natale et al., 2017 ; Koch, 2017 ; Pohlmann et al., 2017 ; Abraham et al., 2018 ; Aithal and Karkou, 2018 ). Figure 3 displays the number of studies that met the inclusion criteria found per year. The search dates were January 2012 to March 2018. While dance and DMT studies were equal in numbers in 2012, there were more DMT studies in 2013–2015, and more dance studies in 2016–2018.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is fpsyg-10-01806-g0003.jpg

Included studies per year with DMT or dance intervention.

Twenty-nine studies (70.73%) implemented the intervention in a clinical sample, and 11 (27.5%) were conducted with a non-clinical (educational) sample. From the clinical populations, 11 were diagnosed with affective disorders (depression), or reported feelings of stress, sadness, or anxiety. Six samples were diagnosed with Parkinson's disease, five with schizophrenia, four with ASD, and two with cognitive impairments. Overall, there were more female than male participants (percentage female participants: M = 65.66%, SD = 27.65). Comparing intervention and control group, the distribution of female and male participants was even.

Interventions

About half of the studies were DMT intervention studies (21 studies); the other half were dance intervention studies (20 studies). In the DMT group , 16 interventions were provided by a trained dance movement therapist, at least four of them were also qualified clinical psychologists. In four trials, descriptions of qualification of therapists were missing or dance movement therapists in training (Aweto et al., 2012 ; Serrano-Guzmán et al., 2016 ; Guerra-Balic et al., 2017 ; Wiedenhofer and Koch, 2017 ). We allocated them to the DMT group because the intervention was predominantly DMT (distinguishing criterion is described above). In the study by Abraham et al. ( 2018 ), classification of qualification of therapist was difficult. The intervention was provided by a physiotherapist specialized at “dynamic neuro-imagery” intervention, a DMT-like introspection intervention. We decided to allocate the study to the DMT group, because of content-related similarities of the intervention to DMT. In the dance intervention group , seven interventions were conducted by an instructor with dance education. Additionally, seven interventions were conducted by exercise instructors with other backgrounds (physical education, physiotherapy, nursing). Six trials contained no description about qualification of dance instructors and were allocated to the cluster because of content-related similarities to the other dance intervention studies.

The DMT studies differed in the technique, e.g., the technique by Marian Chace (“Chacian approach”, a standard method in DMT; Sandel et al., 1993 ), dance therapy with ethno-elements, DMT modified to suit Chinese culture, manualized body psychotherapy (conducted by dance therapists; manual of Röhricht and Papadopolous, unpublished), and dance therapy with elements of creative movement play (for more extensive description of intervention methods in DMT, see Koch, 2019 ). Dance interventions used various dance styles, mostly traditional folk dance or cultural dance forms (Irish set dancing, Greek traditional dance, Flamenco, Poco-Poco dance, Belly dance, Tango), further couple dance (Latin dance), and contemporary dance (jazz, modern, creative dance). All interventions took place in groups. DMT and dance interventions were modified to the age of the participants. In the studies of Meric and Ilhan ( 2016 ) and Panagiotopoulou ( 2018 ), the researcher and the implementing person were identical. The quality of intervention description differed widely across studies. Fifteen percent of the studies described the intervention very rudimentarily, 22.5% moderately, 22.5% more detailed, and 40% replicable, which is a clear increase in replicable descriptions compared to the seven studies in the former meta-analysis by Koch et al. ( 2014 ).

Control Groups

To control for unspecific effects, about one third of the studies (31.7%) used active control groups [e.g., listening to music, cycling, Pilates, physical education/sports exercises (physical exercises), psychoeducation, meditation, relaxation exercises], and 65.9% of the studies used a passive control group (e.g., waiting-list group or treatment as usual). One control group description was missing (Meric and Ilhan, 2016 ).

Duration of Interventions

Most of the included studies were implementing the intervention over a longer period of time (Mean = 11.41 weeks, SD = 7.89, Min = 1 week, Max = 32 weeks). Only the studies by Campion and Levita ( 2014 ) and Wiedenhofer and Koch ( 2017 ) implemented one single-dance/DMT session and assessed short time effects immediately after the intervention. Their sessions lasted for 5 min in the study by Campion and Levita ( 2014 ) and for 50 min in the study by Wiedenhofer and Koch ( 2017 ). The other sessions were usually carried out two times per week (Mean = 1.85, SD = 1.05, Min = once per week, Max = 4 times per week), and lasted for 1 h or 1 1/2 h (Mean = 71.32 min, SD = 19.02, Min = 40 min, Max = 120 min).

Statistical Tests and Assessment Methods

To assess and compare pre–post differences of the dependent variable in the intervention and control group, either t -tests, ANOVAs, or MANOVAs were calculated. Mostly self-report questionnaires were used for assessment (49%), followed by observation rating scales (17.6%), cognitive tests (13.4%), tests of motor functioning (11.1%), interviews (6.5%), and psychophysiological measures (2.3%).

Sample Sizes and Randomization

The meta-analysis included studies with large and very small sample sizes ( N: Mean = 57.90, SD = 49.61, Min = 11, Max = 275). In 21.95% of the cases, the samples were smaller than N = 30, 34.15% were N = 30–50, 31.70% N = 50–100, and 12.20% were larger than N = 100. The studies with the largest sample sizes are contributing most to the results (weighted mean effect sizes); these are (Priebe et al., 2016 ) ( N = 275), (Vankova et al., 2014 ) ( N = 162), (Bräuninger, 2012a ) ( N = 162), (Ho et al., 2016a ) ( N = 139), and (Duberg et al., 2013 ) ( N = 101). In general, smaller studies are less conclusive and generalizable than studies with larger sample sizes (unless their methodological quality is significantly better). Therefore, we weighted their results discriminatingly, calculating weighted mean effect sizes (see previous paragraph). Notably, most studies did not meet established criteria or rules of thumb that would define desirable sample sizes to conclude effects for a certain population (Harris, 1985 ; Kraemer and Thiemann, 1987 ; Green, 1991 ). Nevertheless, it is assumed that (apart from publication bias) studies with a small sample size would not significantly bias the results of meta-analyses but contribute important information, for example, about heterogeneity and the effects in sub-groups (Higgins et al., 2003 ; Grainge, 2015 ).

Randomization is an important criterion for the reliability and validity of estimated effect sizes, because it addresses the issue of comparability of the inspected groups. Fifty-two and a half percent (52.5%) of the studies used randomization as group allocation process, 22.5% used quasi-randomization techniques, and 25% of the studies used no randomization techniques (group allocation was based on self-selection or purposive sampling). Of the 10 non-randomized studies, only 5 included extra assessments of baseline differences, showing that there were significant differences in three of them (Pylvänäinen et al., 2015 ; Adam et al., 2016 ; Aithal and Karkou, 2018 ). The inclusion of studies with baseline differences is justified here, because our methodological approach takes those differences into account when calculating effect sizes. The studies from Biber ( 2016 ), Guerra-Balic et al. ( 2017 ), Meric and Ilhan ( 2016 ), and Panagiotopoulou ( 2018 ) used no statistical tests to assess baseline differences.

Thirty-one of the 41 studies reported dropout rates (Mean = 13.55%, SD = 15.18, Min = 0%, Max = 57.38%). Analysis of dropout rates revealed that 7 of the 31 studies reported a dropout that is higher than 30% (Pinniger et al., 2012 , 2013 ; Röhricht et al., 2013 ; and Rios Romenets et al., 2015 ; Hildebrandt et al., 2016 ; Martin et al., 2016 ; Pohlmann et al., 2017 ). This was in many cases due to the fact that severely impaired patients with schizophrenia or depression had to actively travel to outpatient treatment, requiring a strong motivational state, which is precisely one of the problems in these disorders.

Overall Quality of Included Studies

Because we applied mild selection criteria, the methodological quality of the included primary studies is considerably heterogeneous, and risk of bias is a concern in most of the included studies. Referring to the Cochrane Collaboration's tool for assessing risk of bias in randomized trials, there are six domains of bias that should be considered: selection bias, performance bias, detection bias, attrition bias, reporting bias, and other sources of bias (Higgins et al., 2011 ). Selection bias relates to the group allocation of participants. As reported above, a quarter of the studies had a great risk of selection bias (or in this respect), because no randomization or quasi-randomization tools were used. Additionally, in 12.2% of the trials, no baseline characteristics were checked. Secondly, performance bias relates to blinding of the participants and staff involved. Any of the investigated trials might be affected by this risk of bias (Rosenthal effect), because blinding is a challenge in any type of intervention study. While it is possible to conceal, which is the experimental vs. control condition in active control group trials, it is more difficult in waiting list designs, such as is the case in many of the DMT and dance interventions here. However, it is plausible that self-report measures and external rating scales are more affected by performance bias than cognitive or motor tests or physiological data. Detection bias addresses blinding of the researcher, who assesses and analyses the results. We were not able to extract this information from most of our primary trials, but we assume that researchers usually know the treatment allocation of the participants. Attrition bias is about the issue of the amount and handling of incomplete outcome data. As we assessed dropout rates (see section above), we can state that about 42% of the studies are in danger for this domain of risk of bias. Because selective reporting (reporting bias) is hard to extract from primary trails, we did not assess this domain. Concerning other risks of bias (e.g., conflicts of interest), in two studies, the researcher was also the implementing person (Meric and Ilhan, 2016 ; Panagiotopoulou, 2018 ). To conduct further analyses, we used dichotomous coding (higher-risk vs. lower-risk studies), including all studies in the higher-risk category that had a total N smaller than 30 or which yielded issues in any of the assessed domains of risk of bias (group allocation, attrition bias, other bias). A more detailed description on study characteristics is provided in Table 1 (study characteristics chart).

Overall Effect

Overall, we synthesized data from 2,374 participants from 41 studies (more than twice the number of the last general meta-analysis by Koch et al., 2014 ). According to Cohen ( 1988 ), effect sizes between d = 0.2 and d = 0.5 are small effects, effect sizes between d = 0.5 and d = 0.8 are medium effects, and effect sizes larger than d = 0.8 are large effects. The mean effect sizes per study (Level II, k = 41) varied between one small negative effect size and large positive effect sizes (Unweighted Mean: d = 0.67, Min: d = −0.17, adjusted Max: d = 2.96). The maximum effect size is not bigger than 3.0, because of the adjustments we obtained at Level III. The unadjusted Maximum would be d = 5.07. Further details about the distribution of effect sizes across studies are described in the paragraphs below.

We calculated a fixed-effect model to obtain a weighted mean effect size across studies (Level I). The weighted overall fixed effect was d = 0.48 ( p < 0.001, CI min = 0.40, CI max = 0.57). The analysis of heterogeneity showed a high inconsistency of results ( Q = 127.52, df = 40, p < 0.001, I 2 = 72.62%). Therefore, we calculated a random-effect model as recommended in Lipsey and Wilson ( 2001 ). The estimated weighted overall random effect was d = 0.60 ( p < 0.001, CI min = 0.44, CI max = 0.76). In the following paragraphs, we will only report results that were calculated with the random-effect model.

Effect Sizes According to Outcome Cluster

The weighted mean effects sorted by constructs (outcome clusters) are summarized in the chart below. The analysis yielded a significant effect for each outcome cluster. The effect for interpersonal skills was the largest one, followed by quality of life and (psycho-)motor skills. Furthermore, all effects but the effect for (psycho-)motor skills showed high heterogeneity. Since we calculated a random-effect model, all effects showed broad confidence intervals (see Table 2 ).

Effect sizes according to outcome cluster.

%
Quality of life200.67 0.41–0.990.133<0.00189.30<0.00178.72
Affect230.56 0.34–0.790.115<0.00188.65<0.00175.18
Interpersonal skills90.85 0.41–1.280.222<0.00138.61<0.00178.15
Cognitive skills100.53 0.13–0.930.2040.00928.510.00168.43
(Psycho-)motor skills100.65 0.36–0.960.152<0.00114.610.10238.39
Residual60.47 0.06–0.880.2080.02520.040.00175.05

k, number of studies; ES, effect size; CI, confidence interval; SE, sampling error;

Subanalysis

The subanalysis of anxiety and depression revealed that the differentiation of the two constructs did not lead to more homogeneous results. Depression yielded a slightly larger effect than anxiety. Both effects were medium and showed high heterogeneity. The effect for physiological variables was large but heterogeneous; the effect for positive symptoms in schizophrenia reached significance on the p < 0.1 level and was also heterogeneous (see Table 3 ).

Effect sizes according to sub-clusters.

%
Anxiety90.47 0.09–0.840.1920.01542.88<0.00181.34
Depression180.54 0.30–0.780.124<0.00165.35<0.00173.99
Physiological variables20.88 0.22–10.540.3380.0092.630.10561.98
Schizophrenia (pos symptoms)40.40 −0.01–0.790.2050.057.990.04662.45

The one-session study from Campion and Levita ( 2014 ) was the only one to show a (small) negative effect ( d = −14). Other studies that were at the lower end of the distribution were Koch et al. ( 2015a , b ) that included an anti-violence training ( d = 0.02) and Ho et al. ( 2016a ) that aimed to improve the quality of life of cancer patients ( d = 0.06). On the other side of the distribution, we could observe large effect sizes ( d = 1.8 up to d = 2.96) in the studies of Cruz-Ferreira et al. ( 2015 ), Biber ( 2016 ), Teixeira-Machado et al. ( 2017 ), and Aithal and Karkou ( 2018 ). If we had not recoded the effect sizes from Chiang et al. ( 2016 ), Pohlmann et al. ( 2017 ), Teixeira-Machado et al. ( 2017 ), and Aithal and Karkou ( 2018 ) at Level IV, they would have been even larger (up to d = 5.07). Possible reasons for the effect size distribution across studies are detailed in the next paragraphs.

To analyze the distribution of effect sizes and to address the issue of publication bias, we created a funnel plot (see Figure 4 ) that shows the distribution of unweighted effect sizes as a function of sample sizes. The distribution would be asymmetrical in case of a publication bias, because then small sample studies with positive effect sizes would be published, whereas small sample studies with no positive effects would remain unpublished (Lipsey and Wilson, 2001 ).

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is fpsyg-10-01806-g0004.jpg

Funnel plot.

The diagram in Figure 4 shows a skewed distribution to the right; that is, the large positive effect sizes on the right have no equivalent on the left. This is an indication of a publication bias. Therefore, we conducted a regression analysis (independent variable: sample size, dependent variable: unweighted effect size). There was a small negative gradient parameter that did not reach significance (β = −0.003, p = 0.147). We can thus assume that the publication bias does not have a great impact on the results. Nevertheless, the fact that the gradient parameter did not reach significance does not mean that there is no publication bias at all. We also conducted trim-and-fill analysis as a more elaborate method to explore publication bias using the random-effect model and an L 0 estimator (Duval and Tweedie, 2000 ). The number of estimated missing studies on the left side, meaning the number of studies that are assumed not to be found due to publication bias, was zero. Thus, the new estimated overall effect did not differ from the original one.

As described above, we conducted analysis of sensitivity using study characteristics as metric and categorical moderator of the effect sizes per study ( d i ).

Metric Variables

Regression analyses revealed that no metric variable alone reached significance as a moderator of effect sizes (see Table 4 below). N total was closest to significance, indicating that studies with a greater sample size yielded smaller effects. This can be interpreted as a result of the file-drawer problem, risk of bias of smaller studies, or as an estimator of quality of implementation (we can assume that, in smaller samples, the intervention was implemented more appropriate to the individual) (see Table 4 ).

Metric moderators.

total−0.00250.074
Percentage women (total)0.17390.592
Relation women EG vs. CG0.28610.477
Length process (weeks)0.00960.356
Length session (min)−0.00240.545
Frequency (times per week)0.13750.091
Dropout percentage EG−0.00870.257
Dropout percentage (total)−0.00920.172

EG, experimental group; CG, control group; β, gradient parameter; p, significance parameter .

Overall, 40.84% of variance ( R 2 ) was explained by metric moderators.

Categorical Variables

We conducted sensitivity analysis with the following categorical moderators: country, publication status, higher vs. lower risk of bias, age range, clinical vs. non-clinical sample, diagnosis, DMT vs. dance intervention, qualification of implementing person, quality of intervention description, one-session vs. process, randomization, dropout rates, type of control group activity, and measurement type.

Two moderators were significant on a p < 0.05 level: DMT vs. dance intervention studies (between variance: Q = 5.54, df = 1, p = 0.019), and qualification of implementing person (between variance: Q = 8.89, df = 3, p = 0.031). Trials with DMT interventions yielded significant but slightly lower effect sizes ( d = 0.35, p < 0.001) than trials with dance interventions ( d = 0.81, p < 0.001). In addition, the results of the group with DMT interventions were consistent ( I 2 = 3.47), whereas the results in the dance intervention group were inconsistent ( I 2 = 77.96). Country, higher vs. lower risk of bias, and age range were significant moderators of effect sizes on a p < 0.1 level. In children, teenagers, and seniors, larger effect sizes were observed than in adults, but the effect sizes in adults were more consistent. Furthermore, lower-risk studies yielded smaller effect sizes than higher-risk studies and were more homogeneous. In clinical trials, effect sizes were smaller than in non-clinical trials, but the results remained more consistent than in non-clinical trials (weighted mean effect sizes sorted by group, see Table 5 ).

Categorical moderators.


, , )
, , ) , , ) , , ) , , ) , , ) , , )
Country9.68
5
0.085
Germany (0.46, 0.002, <1%)North/Middle/West
Europe (0.44, 0.028, <1%)
South/East
Europe (0.81, <0.001, 61.26%)
Far East (0.37, 0.142, <1%)USA/Australia/Canada
(0.45, 0.065, 20%)
Others (1.17, <0.001, 61.79%)
Higher vs. lower risk of bias3.27
1
0.071
Lower risk (0.48, 0.000, 8.68%)Higher risk (0.76, 0.000, 34.48%)
Randomization2.27
2
0.320
No randomization
(0.85, <0.001, 44.17%)
Quasi- randomization (0.48, 0.003, <1%)Randomization
(0.57, <0.001, 37.48%)
Age range10.71
5
0.098
Children (1.40, <0.001, 79.51%)Teenager (0.93, 0.003, 79.29%)Young adults (0.55, 0.051, 27.18%)Younger + older adults (0.46, 0.000, <1%)Older adults (0.30, 0.172, <1%)Seniors (0.83, <0.001, 43.75%)
Diagnoses4.17
5
0.654
Cognitive impairment
(0.87, 0.017, 68.75%)
Affective disorders/stress (0.57, <0.001, <1%)Developmental disorders
(0.41, 0.106, <1%)
Schizophrenia (0.40, 0.075, <1%)Parkinson (0.53, 0.033, <1%)None (0.90, <0.001, 72.41%)

df, degree of freedom (df = k – 1) .

Control Group Type

Across all studies, the type of control group activity was not a significant moderator of effect sizes. A different picture resulted, when we only included studies with more than one control group into the analysis ( k = 9, active control groups: meditation, physical exercises). For these studies, the weighted mean effect size remained significant when compared to non-active control groups (in most cases, waiting list CGs), but declined to a visible but non-significant effect when compared to active control groups ( d = 0.43, p = 0.009 vs. d = 0.21, p = 0.126). Especially when compared to meditation intervention, the effects declined to almost zero, which means that the two interventions are equally effective.

Measurement Type

Type of measurement was a significant moderator of effect sizes (between variance: Q = 97.92, df = 5, p < 0.001). Observation rating scales yielded the largest effect sizes ( d = 1.34, p < 0.001, I 2 = 76.01%). They were followed by physiological data ( d = 0.71, p = 0.001, I 2 = n.s.), motor tests ( d = 0.58, p < 0.001, I 2 = n.s.), self-report-questionnaires ( d = 0.42, p < 0.001, I 2 = n.s.), cognitive tests ( d = 0.30, p = 0.002, I 2 = n.s.), and interviews ( d = 0.25, p = 0.001, I 2 = n.s.). We thus can assume that the type of measurement, especially observation rating scales, is one of the most significant sources of heterogeneity across studies. We also checked whether observation rating scales were more frequently used in dance intervention studies than in DMT studies, which was not the case.

Explorative Analyses

Since sensitivity analysis revealed type of intervention as a significant moderator, we decided to exploratively conduct separate analyses of the DMT and dance intervention trials (the results are reported in the Tables 6 , ​ ,7 7 below; they were also reported briefly in the paragraph above).

Effect sizes only in DMT studies.

%
Overall210.35 0.23–0.460.059<0.00120.720.4143.47
Quality of life100.32 0.02–0.630.1550.0361.500.997n.s.
Affect120.51 0.18–0.850.1710.0039.490.577n.s.
Interpersonal skills60.490.00–0.970.2490.0510.430.994n.s.
Cognitive skills30.26*(−0.48)−0.990.2680.0110.230.890n.s.
Motor skills20.30(−0.28)−0.880.2970.3150.140.712n.s.

Effect sizes only in dance intervention studies.

%
Overall210.81 0.52–10.110.149<0.00186.22<0.00177.96
Quality of life101.02 0.69–1.340.165<0.00123.410.00561.55
Affect110.69 0.34–1.040.177<0.00116.630.08339.86
Interpersonal skills31.65 0.90–2.400.383<0.0018.710.01365.57
Cognitive skills70.68 0.16–1.210.268<0.00110.850.09344.65
Motor skills80.76 0.44–1.080.163<0.0018.670.27719.23

We also analyzed weighted mean effect sizes according to outcome clusters separately for the two groups. For quality of life, the effect remained significant in both clusters, but showed consistency only in the DMT group. For clinical outcomes, both effects were significant but heterogeneous. Related to interpersonal skills, there was a significant but inconsistent effect in the dance intervention group and a significant ( p = 0.05) and homogeneous effect in the DMT group. In the cognitive skills cluster, the effect remained significant but heterogeneous in the dance intervention group, but did not reach significance in the DMT group ( k = 3 trials). The effect for motor skills was significant and consistent in the dance intervention group; in the DMT group, it was not significant ( k = 2 trials).

In Tables 6 , ​ ,7, 7 , changes in depression and anxiety are the main outcomes under affect changes . It is notable that the changes in interpersonal skills just barely missed significance in DMT studies. Considering the high heterogeneity of outcome measures in this domain, they need in any case to be further investigated (plus more standardized outcome measured need to be developed). Changes in motor skills are most pronounced and consistent in dance intervention studies, with the most and the most rigorous evidence from studies of interventions for Parkinson patients (e.g., Hackney and Bennett, 2014 ; Sharp and Hewitt, 2014 ; Loetzke et al., 2015 ). Changes in motor skills are usually not in the focus of DMT studies. The non-significant results on motor skills confirm DMT as a psychotherapeutic intervention.

Assessment of Follow-Up Data

In order to obtain information about the long-term effects of DMT and dance interventions, we analyzed the available follow-up data (see Table 8 ). Eight studies reported follow-up data, the mean period from post- to follow-up assessment was 22 weeks. In most studies, the effect remained constant. The largest decline was observed in the study by Bräuninger ( 2012a , b ). The effect was reduced by half, but remained significant. In Baptista et al. ( 2012 ), Pylvänäinen et al. ( 2015 ) and Priebe et al. ( 2016 ), there was a slight decline, which was close to zero; in De Natale et al. ( 2017 ), there was a slight increase. In the studies from Pinniger et al. ( 2012 ), Duberg et al. ( 2013 ), and Cruz-Ferreira et al. ( 2015 ), there was a large increase (e.g., in Pinniger the effect tripled). The authors explain this unusual increase with an increase in mindfulness (detachment from negative thoughts, ruminations, and worries, which led to a reduction in symptom severity; Ree and Craigie, 2007 ) and a personal bond between the participants as a learning process, which became more effective after the participants had practiced for a while.

Effect sizes follow-up.

d ) d ) d ) )
Baptista et al. ( ) -D320.480.39−0.0918.22
Bräuninger ( , )240.640.38−0.2637.12
Cruz-Ferreira et al. ( ) -D242.543.000.467.83
De Natale et al. ( )80.250.390.143.91
Duberg et al. ( ) -D480.510.810.3024.40
Pinniger et al. ( )40.270.820.5511.59
Priebe et al. ( )240.190.14−0.0568.43
Pylvänäinen et al. ( )120.740.65−0.097.18
Weighted mean ( )220.67 (0.001)0.79 (0.001)0.05 (0.613)

The bottom line displays the weighted mean effect sizes across the eight studies and a significance parameter (p-value); D, dance intervention study .

Summary of Results

In this meta-analysis, we investigated the effectiveness of DMT and dance interventions on health-related psychological outcomes. We included 41 primary trails published between 2012 and March of 2018 that contained a total of 2,374 participants. Twenty-one of these trials considered DMT interventions, whereas 20 trials considered dance interventions. We grouped the dependent variables into six outcome clusters: quality of life ( k = 20 trials), clinical outcomes (23 trials; sub-analysis: depression, anxiety), interpersonal skills ( k = 9 trials), cognitive skills ( k = 10 trials), (psycho-)motor skills ( k = 10 trials), and residuals ( k = 6 trials; physiological data, positive symptoms schizophrenia). The overall mean effect size was d = 0.60 ( p < 0.001, CI min = 0.44, CI max = 0.76), which is a significant medium effect (Cohen, 1988 ). Analysis of heterogeneity yielded that an estimated 71.62% of variance of results can be traced back to dissimilarity of results, which makes the interpretation of results more difficult. In the outcome clusters, we also obtained significant medium effect sizes [quality of life: d = 0.67, affect: d = 0.56, cognitive skills: d = 0.53, (psycho-)motor skills: d = 0.65, residuals d = 0.47] and one significant large effect size (interpersonal skills: d = 0.85). This could be due to the fact that in the interpersonal skills cluster, many trials assessed dependent variables with observation rating scales, which yielded larger effect sizes in general. Sub-analyses revealed that the mean effect in the anxiety cluster was as large as the effect in the depression cluster. Physiological data yielded a mean large, significant effect size, whereas improvements of schizophrenia yielded a mean small effect size, which reached the p < 0.1 level. Assessment of heterogeneity in the outcome clusters revealed that all mean effects, except the effect for (psycho-)motor skills, remained inconsistent (i.e., significant Q, I 2 larger 60%). Furthermore, the larger the mean effect, the larger the heterogeneity of results, which indicates that larger effects were produced by outliers rather than by consistently higher effect sizes (see end of the next paragraph).

In order to identify which study characteristics contributed most to dissimilarities of results, we conducted a sensitivity analysis. Most importantly, we found that the type of intervention (DMT vs. dance) was a significant moderator of results. Therefore, it was reasonable to analyze data in two separate groups to obtain more meaningful results:

In the DMT group , we obtained smaller but more consistent effects. The overall medium effect was small, significant, and consistent/homogeneous ( d = 0.30, p < 0.001, non-significant Q, I 2 = 3.47). For the individual outcomes, the effects varied between d = 0.26 and d = 0.51 and were all homogeneous. The effects for quality of life, affect, and cognitive skills remained significant, whereas the effect for interpersonal skills reached the p < 0.1 level. Effects of motor skills were non-significant, thus confirming DMT as a mainly psychotherapeutic intervention.

In the dance cluster , we observed larger but less consistent effects. The overall medium effect was large, significant but non-consistent/heterogeneous ( d = 0.81, p < 0.001, significant Q, I 2 = 77.96). The effects in the outcome cluster varied between d = 0.68 and d = 1.65. They were all significant, but only the effects for (psycho-)motor skills, cognitive skills, and affect were consistent (non-significant Q, I 2 < 50%). Apart from Koch et al. ( 2015a , b ) and Aithal and Karkou ( 2018 ), all outliers we mentioned in the “Analysis of outliers and publication bias” section were part of the dance intervention group. The consistent effects for (psycho)motor skills were carried by the dance for Parkinson studies.

Dance studies seem to uphold certain characteristics that produce a broader range of results, especially in the large-positive spectrum of effect sizes (i.e., greater than d = 1.0) that are not evident in DMT studies. One factor might be different sample characteristics: Most DMT studies were conducted in a clinical setting, whereas most dance studies were conducted in a non-clinical setting. In severely impaired patient samples, such as in most of the DMT primary studies that entered into this analysis, effects are usually smaller than in non-clinical or subclinical populations (we know this, for example, from pretests of designs with student populations; e.g., Koch, 2011 ). This may be a major reason why the dance intervention studies have yielded larger medium effect sizes. Non-clinical samples also contained a broader age range. Additionally, in dance interventions, the implementation and methods were more heterogeneous than in the DMT group. In terms of culture, most DMT studies were conducted in Germany and other Western European countries, whereas the majority of dance intervention studies were conducted in non-Western countries. Dance intervention studies tended to have smaller sample sizes, less randomization, and more missing information in the reporting of results.

In sum, we obtained encouraging results, which indicated that DMT and dance have positive effects on various health-related outcomes. Most studies found evidence on the effectiveness of DMT on clinical outcomes ( k = 12 trials), followed by quality of life ( k = 10 trials) and cognitive skills ( k = 3 trials). There was also a tendency that DMT improved interpersonal skills ( k = 6 trials; p = 0.051). We did not find enough studies on (psycho-)motor functioning, physiological changes, and positive symptoms of schizophrenia in the DMT cluster to draw conclusions. Dance interventions improved (psycho-)motor skills ( k = 8 trials), clinical outcomes ( k = 11 trials), and cognitive skills ( k = 7 trials). The high variety of results, especially in the dance cluster, needs more investigation in future studies.

Specific Research Issues and Practical Implications for Researchers

In our meta-analysis, we also assessed and analyzed various study characteristics to deepen our understanding of factors that influenced the effects of DMT and dance on health-related outcomes. One important issue in most intervention studies is the question about unspecific and specific effects of the intervention . Often the fact that we pay attention to the participants already can change their symptoms (Hawthorne effect: firstly described in Roethlisberger and Dickson ( 1964 ), and reviewed by McCambridge et al., 2014 ). Using alternative interventions in the control groups helps to distinguish attention effects from specific effects. Therefore, we assessed control group activity. In the primary trials, one third of the DMT and dance intervention groups were compared to active control groups. The participants followed the following tasks: listening to music, cycling, Pilates, physical exercises, psychoeducation, meditation, or relaxation exercises. Control group activity was not a significant moderator of effect, which means that the effects of DMT and dance interventions were still evident when we controlled for attention effects. In other words, this indicates that there are specific effects of DMT and dance interventions. Control group activity seemed to matter, when we kept all other study characteristics constant, which was only possible in studies with an active and a non-active control group ( k = 9).

We compared the intervention groups to control groups that participated in physical exercises and meditation exercises. The effects of DMT and dance interventions declined but remained significant. The advantages of DMT and dance interventions were larger compared to physical exercises than to meditation. This indicates that the change mechanisms of DMT and dance possibly lie beyond the pure execution of sports or mindfulness. This is in line with other research on the mechanisms of change in DMT and dance (Bräuninger, 2014 ; Koch, 2017 ). The effect sizes observed in DMT and dance intervention groups were about as large as the effect sizes in meditation interventions. There is plenty of evidence that meditation interventions, for example, training programs in mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR; Teasdale et al., 2000 ; Michalak et al., 2008 ), are successful in treating several psychological conditions (e.g., depression, anxiety, stress, obsessive–compulsive disorders; Bohlmeijer et al., 2010 ; Fjorback et al., 2011 ). On basis of this evidence, there is also an increase in promotion and support of mindfulness-based interventions by the health insurance companies, such as free MBSR training programs at work. Thus, observing that DMT and dance interventions seem to be as effective as meditation suggests promoting these kinds of interventions as well. One interesting research issue that we should investigate in more detail is: What do DMT/dance interventions and mindfulness-based interventions have in common? Mindfulness is an important component of DMT. For example, mindful investigation of body sensations is part of DMT, with similar techniques as the body-scan exercise in MBSR training programs (Dreeben et al., 2013 ). Besides that, getting mindfully in contact with other people (therapist or other group members) is an important mechanism of change in DMT. Concerning dance interventions, there is less explicit use of mindfulness, but if we look closer, similar mechanisms of change stand out. One central component frequently mentioned in the literature on mindfulness is the termination of rumination and automatic negative thoughts, which occurs, because there is high concentration, focus on the here-and-now, and a state of mind, which allows the participant to observe feelings and thoughts from a more distant perspective (non-judgmental state of mind). Future research should investigate to what extent these mechanisms are relevant in DMT and dance intervention and how they influence the outcomes of the interventions. Moreover, we assume that an important overarching therapeutic factor in both mindfulness practice and DMT could be introspection (Price and Smith-DiJulio, 2016 ). Another central mechanism of change in dance and DMT might be the experience of flow (Csikszentmihalyi and Csikszentmihalyi, 1975 ). It is based on the same principles as mindfulness, such as high concentration, absorption, focus on the here-and-now, physical presence, and joy (Csikszentmihalyi and Csikszentmihalyi, 1975 ).

In order to find out which populations DMT and dance interventions are indicated for, we investigated the influence of sample characteristics on the effect sizes. Age range and clinical vs. non-clinical populations were two correlating moderators on a p < 0.1 level. We observed larger mean effects in children and elderly than in younger and older adults. At the same time, there was higher heterogeneity of results in the group of children and elderly. The fact that outcome variables in children and elderly were assessed with observation rating scales more often might contribute to these differences. Furthermore, the larger effects in children may also have come about because there were more prevention studies or studies in educational context with children and older adults, whereas the adults were mostly severely impaired clinical populations. Prevention studies yielded larger effects but higher heterogeneity than clinical studies. Again, this means that rather than more pronounced effects, there are outliers at the large-positive spectrum of effect sizes. From these results, we cannot come to recommendations about the indication of treatment in certain populations. Further research is needed to ascertain moderators that produce the variety of results. Regarding outcomes characteristics , DMT is indicated when a psychological change is intended, whereas dance interventions are indicated to improve motor skills (the consistent effect only occurred on those).

The so-called method effect (“any characteristic of measurement procedure contributes to variance of scores”; Maul, 2013 ) is a widely discussed limitation of intervention studies. Thus, we assessed and analyzed methodological characteristics of studies. We found one significant moderator, type of measurement , and two moderators that reached a p < 0.1 level, country of publication and methodological quality of study (risk of bias, randomization, sample size). Observation rating scales yielded the largest effect sizes followed by physiological data, motor tests, self-report questionnaires, cognitive tests, and interviews. Some measurements may be superior to detect changes or more sensitive to certain risks of bias, therefore producing larger effects than others. For example, observation rating scales and self-report questionnaires are more likely to be affected by expectation effects, which will be discussed further in the limitations of the present study paragraph. However, it is important to mention that method effects can also systematically bias the results. There was also a correlation between country of publication and methodological characteristics of the trials (risk of bias, randomization, sample size), with both factors having a slight influence on the observed effects. This is not surprising, because standards and traditions of research vary between countries. In countries with less evidence-based research traditions, methodological standards tend to be less strict and resulting effects tend to be larger. However, the effect of country could also be related to other cultural factors such as experience with the specific dance form, or the (sub-)cultural value system around dance, which may all influence motivation for and responsiveness to treatment.

We also analyzed whether the intensity of treatment had an influence on effect sizes. The intensity of treatment was indicated by three factors: duration of the whole treatment, duration of one session, and frequency of treatment. None of these factors reached significance. Frequency of treatment was closest to significance (β = 0.1375, p = 0.091), indicating that higher frequency of treatment corresponded with slightly higher effect sizes.

In the last paragraphs, we discussed moderators that showed a significant influence on the effects of DMT and dance in our sensitivity analysis. However, we assume that there are more potentially important moderators of effects , which we were not able to address in our analysis. In the literature, there are factors that we can allocate to four clusters: (a) factors that relate to characteristics of the participants, (b) factors that relate to characteristics of the intervention, (c) factors that relate to characteristics of the implementing person, and (d) factors of environment and factors that relate to the relation between persons involved.

Firstly, we will discuss characteristics of participants. Savill et al. ( 2017 ) stated that gender is an important moderator for the effectiveness of body psychotherapy on negative symptoms in schizophrenia. They performed a secondary analysis with the data of a large multicenter randomized controlled trial by Priebe et al. ( 2016 ), the so-called “NESS paper.” The interaction between gender and treatment allocation as a predictor of outcomes was examined in 275 participants (72 women and 203 men) randomized to either a body psychotherapy or a Pilates group (for a critique of the study, particularly its control group selection, see the last paragraph of the discussion on the need for mechanism studies). Negative symptoms in schizophrenia “were found to significantly decrease in women randomized to the body psychotherapy condition in comparison to Pilates, while no such effect was detected in men” (Savill et al., 2017 , p. 1). To approach this issue, we also assessed percentages of female participants in intervention and control groups in our meta-analysis. We conducted a sensitivity analysis using the total percentage of female participants and the relation between female and male participants of intervention group and control group as a moderator. We found no significant influence of gender. This type of analysis has low power, which means that there is a high risk for a β-error. Thus, future research needs to investigate whether gender is an important moderator of effects. Further potential participant-related factors are self-efficacy or outcome expectations (Murrock and Madigan, 2008 ), attitude toward intervention (treatment adherence, motivation), and previous dance experiences. Regarding characteristics of DMT interventions, there is a high variety of methods and therapeutic styles, because only a few of the trials included manualized implementation of treatment (Martin et al., 2016 ; Priebe et al., 2016 ). Dance instructors used many different dance styles and teaching approaches. Although it was not possible in our meta-analysis, it would be useful to investigate differences between the effects of several methods and styles in the DMT and dance spectrum, in order to gain more knowledge about differential indications (Koch, 2019 ) and therapeutic factors. In her meta-analysis, Peters ( 2012 ) found qualification of therapist to be a significant moderator of effects. Concerning environmental factors, she discussed the influence of social support . Murrock and Madigan ( 2008 ) found that social support from friends mediated the effect between culturally specific dance and lifestyle physical activity. Finally, as in most therapeutic interventions, the relationship between the therapist and the participant is important (Grawe et al., 1994 ; Wampold and Irmel, 2015 ). As previously mentioned in the Introduction, the relationship between participants (group cohesion) is also assumed to be an important mediator of effects (Schmais, 1985 , 1998 ; Yalom, 1985 ) in interventions that are conducted in a group setting.

It is always difficult to observe long-term effects in meta-analyses. In our sample, only eight of the included trials reported follow-up data (mean period: 22 weeks). The analyses yielded various results, but most effects remained constant or increased. Regarding the encouraging follow-up findings, we assume that DMT and dance interventions have the potential to initiate a learning process (body access, interoception, insight) that might instigate positive changes several months after the intervention. Additionally, it is plausible that the permanence of effects depends on the participants' behavior after the intervention, such as revising what they have learned, continuing dance or movement classes, or maintaining contact with the other participants. Further research needs to follow to investigate long-term effects and their moderators.

Limitations

One of the biggest limitations of our present study was the heterogeneity of results, which occurred due to mild inclusion criteria and various study characteristics of the included trials. The heterogeneity mostly concerned the dance intervention studies, whereas the DMT studies were rather homogeneous. Heterogeneity was caused, for example, by dissimilarities in methodological designs of studies, methods of interventions, and sample characteristics. Besides that, we observed effects on several different health-related psychological outcomes. For this reason, we employed a random-effect model, which considers that not all studies measure the same effect. Consequently, we obtained broad 95% confidence intervals, meaning that the “true effects” could also be much larger or smaller than the weighted mean effect sizes reported in this paper (e.g., some of the confidence intervals varied between no effect and medium effects or small effects and large effects). Such results are less conclusive. This leads to a typical critique of meta-analysis as “ comparing apples and oranges ” (Sharpe, 1997 ), which means that non-comparable outcomes are unjustifiably compared with each other. Yet, in cases where there is not a lot of evidence, it might be a useful start, if we want to obtain information about “fruits”—to stay in the metaphor. However, we should be aware that we lose specific information about discrete sorts of fruits and mostly create a starting point to generate useful hypotheses for future, more specific secondary analyses and primary trials.

Our meta-analysis is one of the first in the field to conduct sensitivity analysis , which is one approach to deepen the understanding about the sources of heterogeneity. Nevertheless, it is impossible to detect all important factors and to draw firm conclusions about causal relationships between those factors. The strength of our paper is that it provides a broad overview of current research on the therapeutic use of dance as an orientation for researchers (summarize findings, identify explanatory variables, help identifying research gaps and develop research questions, control standards of research). It informedly transfers knowledge about the effectiveness of DMT and dance interventions to practitioners, clients, and public decision-makers. However, the disadvantage of broad analyses is that we only obtain results for a rough orientation. It is the assignment of secondary analyses with more narrow research questions (such as provided in Cochrane Reviews for single clinical populations) to gain sharp and more detailed knowledge about the effectiveness of DMT and dance interventions and the interdependency with contributing contextual factors.

Another issue is the so-called “garbage-in–garbage-out problem,” meaning that the results are less conclusive if we include primary outcome trials with poor methodological quality. In the present study, we also included studies with considerable methodological constraints (e.g., small N , no randomization, high dropout, deficient report of implementation or statistics, conflicts of interest). The most important question is how much the methodological constraints may systematically bias the results reported in this paper (the weighted medium effect sizes). We approached the methodological variety of primary trials with sensitivity analysis. There was a tendency that more outliers, especially with large effect sizes, were studies with more severe methodological constraints. “Higher vs. lower risk studies” was a moderator of effects on a p < 0.1 level. The medium effect size in “lower risk studies” was d = 0.48 ( p < 0.001, I 2 = 8.68%), which is slightly lower and more homogeneous than the overall weighted mean effect size ( d = 0.60, p = 0.001, I 2 = 72.62%). This might be a hint that studies with more significant methodological constraints tend to overestimate the effects of DMT and dance. Since all of these problems were more pronounced in the dance intervention studies, one resulting recommendation is to separate DMT and dance intervention studies in the next general meta-analysis.

One important source of bias might be the “Rosenthal effect” or expectancy effect , which means that the expectations of the researcher are subtly communicated to the participants. By guessing the goal of the research, participants try to comply with its assumed goal (Rosenthal, 1966 ). Thus, self-report-questionnaires or observation rating scales are more prone to bias than cognitive, physiological, or motor test, which are somewhat less subjective, but still reactive (note that the reactiveness of measurement type ranked differently in our study as indicated above). Furthermore, it is possible that researchers tend to analyze, interpret, and report results in favor of positive effects, because of their own expectancies or potential conflicts of interest.

Another critical element that might contribute to the fact that higher-risk studies yielded larger effect sizes could be publication bias . One criterion of methodological quality of study was sample size. If there is publication bias, smaller studies yield larger effects, on average, because smaller studies, which did not detect positive effects, remained unpublished. In the distribution of our sample of primary trials, there was a small tendency for publication bias, which did not affect our results significantly (see Methods section). Furthermore, smaller studies yielding larger effects could also be explained by the fact that, in smaller samples, the intervention was more tailored to the individual and therefore more focused.

Apart from quality of included trials, quantity of studies is also a matter in meta-analysis. Compared to Koch et al. ( 2014 ), we obtained larger analysis clusters; however, especially for interpersonal skills, cognitive skills, and (psycho-)motor outcomes, more research is needed to obtain more meaningful results. The advantage of a bigger sample per outcome would be that more homogeneous clusters could be considered and addressed with sensitivity analysis. In addition, several included trials were conducted by the same research group (total: 7 of 41 trials by Koch et al., 2019 ), which is a threat to external validity. This is particularly relevant, when it comes to analysis of clusters (DMT group: 7 of 21 trials, clinical outcome cluster: 4 of 12 trials, interpersonal skills trials: 2 of 6 studies by the research group of Koch et al., 2019 ).

Finally, there is a general discussion on the issue of whether quantitative analyses are the appropriate means to evaluate the therapeutic use of dance. Borg ( 1993 ) stated that the dilemma of scientific (positivistic) research in behavioral sciences is that it applies traditional concepts of physical science to the study of living organisms, although living organisms are far more complex than physical objects of study. The authors imply that naturally dynamic, interdependent networks of factors involved in psychological phenomena are sometimes hard to detect when we apply concepts of causality, predictability, and scientific reducibility. This argument is underlined by a vivid discussion on the ecological validity of studies reducing complex processes, such as an aesthetic experience or the impact of art perception and production on health, into its single components (see e.g., Christensen and Jola, 2015 ). While quantitative methods are helpful for generating facts and explanations, qualitative methods might be more suitable for meaning-making and understanding of such (Berrol, 2000 ). In order to overcome the gaps between a non-linear reality and linear means of investigation, and to generate new scientific insights, quantitative research ought to be applied together with qualitative research, ideally in mixed-methods designs that reflect the epistemological background assumptions of the studied processes.

Hervey ( 2000 ) takes the discussion a step further by stating that artistic inquiry is needed to adequately reflect the results of such process-oriented domains as DMT. She introduces the concept of artistic inquiry, as part of arts-based research to DMT, which implies the use of the respective art form (dance) not only as an intervention to help the recipient but also as a form of data assessment, analysis, and presentation that aims at answering the research question. Leavy's textbook (2017) is the basic source for arts-based research today, providing a terminological and historical overview and best practice examples of arts-based research. Best practice examples of specific and particularly well-described arts-based research methods are Jola's embodied neuroscience (Jola, 2013 ), and Eberhard's aesthetic answering (Lange et al., in press ), both participatory approaches with the researcher in an active embodied role, diving into non-verbal processes to inform and answer the research question. However, at this point, final recommendations about arts-based research as a method are difficult, because the field is young and in a dynamic development (Leavy, 2017 ). Generally, non-verbal methods employing DMT or dance may detect changes in psychological outcomes that are not necessarily accessible with traditional methods and thus create innovative knowledge.

Recommendations for Future Research

This meta-analysis shows that quantitative research on the therapeutic use of dance is augmenting. However, there is still an urgent quest for more trials with rigorous standards in respect to the chosen way of research (quantitative, qualitative, arts-based). Quantitative trials should consider larger sample sizes, randomized controlled designs, and active control groups, which compare DMT and dance interventions to other psychotherapeutic interventions with existing knowledge about therapeutic mechanisms (different types of psychodynamic therapies, cognitive behavioral therapy, pharmacological therapy). Furthermore, a detailed description of the intervention and its implementation is necessary (e.g., for replications). To avoid bias, there should be as much blinding as possible involved in the process. While complete blinding is not possible in therapy studies (and should thus not be part of the quality assessment of therapeutic trials to add this critique here), blinding of the randomization process and the accessor should be standard. Researchers ought to employ assessment tools that are least sensitive to expectation effects (e.g., standardized tests, psychophysiological measurement). All relevant treatment conditions should be reported in as much detail as possible (e.g., sample characteristics, characteristics of therapists, dropouts, structure and content of the treatment, other therapies provided, and interim circumstances, i.e., all “external” events that occurred in the time of the treatment, such as change of partner or job, etc.). Statistical results should be reported in detail including results that were not in line with the central hypotheses of the papers and descriptive statistics (means, standard deviations, and sample sizes in each group). To provide information on the impact of a treatment, long-term effects must be considered. Therefore, we highly recommend including follow-up assessment in intervention studies on the therapeutic use of DMT and dance.

In order to obtain more comparable research across the globe, there should be more communication between researchers, and they should strive for international standards. We recommend researchers conducting future meta-analyses to include more precise assessment of risk of bias, than was possible here. In addition, systematic analyses of moderators of effects should be performed in the future. Because we observed many dissimilarities between DMT and dance intervention studies, we recommend analysing those two types of studies separately in future studies.

Finally, to draw conclusions for practice, there is a need to complement quantitative research inquiry with qualitative and arts-based research (best in mixed-methods designs, reflecting the epistemological framework) and with clinical mechanism studies.

Mechanism Research Needs to Inform Outcome Research

The urgent need for mechanism studies (Kazdin, 2007 ) and their interdependency with outcome studies shall here be exemplified with the debate around the included “NESS paper” (Priebe et al., 2016 ). Priebe et al. ( 2016 ) tested the decrease of negative symptoms in patients with schizophrenia after movement therapy (i.e., body psychotherapy, BPT, conducted by dance movement therapists; for definition of these terms, see Martin et al., 2016 ) in a randomized sample of N = 275 participants. They found that “the adjusted difference in negative symptoms was 0.03 (95% CI −1.11 to 1.17), indicating no benefit from body psychotherapy. Small improvements in expressive deficits and movement disorder symptoms were detected in favor of body psychotherapy. No other outcomes were significantly different.” With the interpretation of these findings, the authors question other relevant studies of the field, including their own earlier work (see Röhricht and Priebe, 2006 ; Lee et al., 2015 ; and Martin et al., 2016 —the latter a study from the same year, employing the same treatment manual (Röhricht and Papadopolous, unpublished). with a TAU control group, showing a significant reduction of negative affect after DMT (measured with the SANS). The NESS paper's data were mixed though, and further analyses in the secondary trial of Savill et al. ( 2017 ) have shown that the null effect discussed in the NESS paper was only true for men, not for women after the intervention.

Three arguments call for the necessity of a reappraisal of the NESS paper: the problematic domain overgeneralization from DMT/BPT to all arts therapies (domain and terminology-related aspect), the control group selection (DMT/BPT vs. Pilates), and the selective reporting and shortfall in conclusions, which do not appropriately reflect what is evidenced in the data (e.g., from the measures of negative symptoms, the PANSS showed no significant difference between groups, whereas the CAINS did; SANS, PANSS, and CAINS are all standardized observational measures to assess positive and negative symptoms of schizophrenia). In the context of the mechanism problem, we will merely discuss the second argument here.

Active control groups are recommended for most studies by the increasing standards of the evidence-based medicine. Priebe et al. ( 2016 ) thus tried to implement a suitable active control group. From the perspective of DMT though, with its present pronounced research on therapeutic mechanisms, Pilates is not a suited control group to DMT. Both interventions, DMT/BPT and Pilates, employ methods that are suited to increase body awareness. In Pilates, the torso is the focus of the work, the muscle tone is actively controlled and altered in specific regions of the torso, the muscles are strengthened and stretched, and the practice includes breath work for bringing the movements of the torso in resonance with the breath. Pilates had the goal of addressing the trinity of body, mind, and spirit in a holistic way (“Return to Life through Contrology,” Pilates and Miller, 1945 ). In DMT theory, the torso is the seat of the emotions, and the breath brings the emotions to the fore: on the basis of DMT core knowledge, the work with the torso and breath is the direct pathway to sensation, experience, and expression of emotions (e.g., Caldwell, 1996 ). Thus, the resulting null findings on the PANSS are not surprising. While we know very little about the mechanisms of BPT and DMT, we know even less about the mechanisms of Pilates and other body practices. Thus, it is very difficult, if not impossible, to select suitable active control groups for DMT studies, without any knowledge of either intervention's main mechanisms. This example shows that good outcome research needs scholarly mechanisms research (Hayes, 2013 ) and that there is a strong interdependency between these two types of clinical research.

Because mechanisms of DMT/BPT are not well-researched, and even less so mechanisms of Pilates, both the experimental group and the control group may have experienced similar working mechanisms, which may have caused the inconclusive results. As long as the major mechanisms of these therapies remain unclear, it is hard to draw any valid conclusions from the according outcome research. Thus, the effect of DMT/BPT on the reduction of negative symptoms needs to be investigated with a range of control groups. Primarily therapeutic mechanisms of DMT/BPT and potential control interventions, respectively, need to be further investigated, before conducting another primary study of the scope of the NESS paper. With this paragraph, we hope to have illustrated the urgent need for mechanisms studies due to their intricate interdependency with outcome research.

In conclusion, the results of our meta-analysis suggest that therapeutic use of dance potentially affects various health-related psychological outcomes. In total, there was a medium significant overall effect based on heterogeneous results. However, since type of intervention was a significant source of heterogeneity, we explored trials on DMT and trials on dance interventions in two separate groups. We found empirical evidence that DMT consistently and with a high homogeneity improved affect-related psychological conditions by decreasing anxiety and depression levels, and increased quality of life and cognitive skills. Concerning interpersonal skills, the effect reached the p < 0.1 level. More high-quality primary studies need to be conducted and included into meta-analyses to expand the evidence. Dance intervention studies consistently improved motor skills, while findings for the other outcomes had a high heterogeneity. Results of this meta-analysis suggest that DMT and dance interventions improve clinical outcomes, cognitive outcomes, and (psycho-)motor outcomes. The high variability of results, especially in the dance cluster, needs further attention. Moreover, this study contributes initial findings that DMT and dance interventions have persistent long-term effects. These encouraging results are limited by methodological shortcomings of the primary studies. Further research is needed that expands on the evidence of effects of DMT and dance interventions on health-related psychological outcomes.

Author Contributions

SK conceived, planned, co-wrote and revised the study and supervised the Master's (RR, KT) and doctoral level students (LM, JB). LM and KT did the systematic literature search. LM, KT, and SK did the hand search. RR and AB planned and implemented the methodological approach, AB supervised the methodology of the study. KT, JB, and RR organized the results. RR analyzed the results and wrote the first draft. All authors contributed to the paper and revised it into its final version.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Simea Schönenberger, Friederike Klodwig, and Katrin Sachau for helping to extract data from the primary studies as part of their DMT student elective hours. We would like to thank all colleagues who have supported us to find relevant literature, and all authors of primary studies who helped to clarify data issues. Thanks to all participants of the primary studies for completing the scales and driving research on embodied interventions further.

1 This term includes the practice of dance movement psychotherapy (UK terminology) and dance/movement therapy (US terminology).

2 For a list of abbreviations, see Appendix .

3 Creative Arts Therapies (European term: Arts Therapies; Karkou and Sanderson, 2006 ) consist mainly of art therapy, music therapy, dance movement therapy, drama therapy, and poetry therapy/expressive writing.

References marked with an asterisk indicate studies that are included in the meta-analysis; references marked with a circle were screened but excluded studies.

Funding. The study was conducted by the Research Institute for Creative Arts Therapies (RIArT) at Alanus University funded by the Software AG Foundation in Darmstadt, in colaboration with the University of Jena, Drexel University, and SRH University Heidelberg. The Faculty of Therapy Sciences at SRH University Heidelberg funded the publication.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01806/full#supplementary-material

  • ° Alotaibi A. Karkou V. van Der Linden M. L. Irvine L. M. C (2017). Movement therapy programme with children with mild learning difficulties in primary schools in Saudi Arabia: links between motion and emotion, in The Oxford Handbook of Dance and Wellbeing , eds Karkou V., Oliver S., Lycouris S. (Edinburgh: Queen Margaret University, 479–492. [ Google Scholar ]
  • ° Alrazain B., Zubala A., Karkou V. (2018). Movement-based arts therapy for children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in the Kingdom of Saudi Arabia, in Arts Therapies in the Treatment of Depression , eds Zubala A., Karkou V. (London: Routledge, 84–100. [ Google Scholar ]
  • ° Burzynska A. Z., Jiao Y., Knecht A. M., Fanning J., Awick E. A., Chen T., et al. (2017). White matter integrity declined over 6-months, but dance intervention improved integrity of the Fornix of older adults . Front. Aging Neurosci. 9 :59 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00059 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • ° Cross K., Flores R., Butterfield J., Blackman M., Lee S. (2012). The effect of passive listening versus active observation of music and dance performances on memory recognition and mild to moderate depression in cognitively impaired older adults . Psychol. Rep. 111 , 413–423. 10.2466/10.02.13.PR0.111.5.413-423 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • ° Koch S., Kelbel J., Kolter A., Sattel H., Fuchs T. (2017). (Dis)embodiment in schizophrenia: effects of mirroring of self-experience, empathy and wellbeing, in Oxford Handbook Dance Wellbeing , eds Karkou V., Oliver S., Lycouris S. (London, UK: Routledge; ), 863–882. [ Google Scholar ]
  • ° Lewis C., Annett L. E., Davenport S., Hall A. A., Lovatt P. (2014). Mood changes following social dance sessions in people with Parkinson's disease . J. Health Psychol. 21 , 483–492. 10.1177/1359105314529681 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • ° Mateos-Moreno D., Atencia-Doña L. (2013). Effect of a combined dance/movement and music therapy on young adults diagnosed with severe autism . Arts Psychother. 40 , 465–472. 10.1016/j.aip.2013.09.004 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • ° Meyer DeMott M. A., Jakobsen M., Wentzel-Larsen T., Heir T. (2017). A controlled early group intervention study for unaccompanied minors: can expressive arts alleviate symptoms of trauma and enhance life satisfaction? Scand. J. Psychol. 58 , 510–518. 10.1111/sjop.12395 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • ° Savill M., Orfanos S., Bentall R., Reininghaus U., Wykes T., Priebe S. (2017). The impact of gender on treatment effectiveness of body psychotherapy for negative symptoms of schizophrenia: a secondary analysis of the NESS trial data . Psychiatry Res. 247 , 73–78. 10.1016/j.psychres.2016.11.020 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • ° Vancea F. (2013). Unifying personal development through dance, movement and the increase of the emotional intelligence level . J. Exp. Psychother. Psihoter. Exp. 16 :3. [ Google Scholar ]
  • * . Abraham A., Hart A., Andrade I., Hackney M. E. (2018). Dynamic neuro-cognitive imagery improves mental imagery ability, disease severity, and motor and cognitive functions in people with Parkinson's disease . Neural Plast. 2018 :6168507. 10.1155/2018/6168507 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • * . Adam D., Ramli A., Shahar S. (2016). Effectiveness of a combined dance and relaxation intervention on reducing anxiety and depression and improving quality of life among the cognitively impaired elderly . Sultan Qaboos Univ. Med. J. 16 , 47–53. 10.18295/squmj.2016.16.01.009 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • ADTA American Dance Therapy Association (2018). Available online at: www.adta.org
  • Aguiar L. P. C., Da Rocha P. A., Morris M. (2016). Therapeutic dancing for Parkinson's disease . Int. J. Gerontol . 10 , 64–70. 10.1016/j.ijge.2016.02.002 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • * . Aithal S., Karkou V. (2018). Backing the backbones—a feasibility study of effectiveness of dance movement psychotherapy on parenting stress in caregivers of children with Autism Spectrum Disorders . Arts Psycother. 64 , 69–76. 10.1016/j.aip.2019.04.003 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • * . Allet L., Müller-Pinget S., Punt I., Edelsten C., Ballif A., Golay A., et al.. (2017). Dance therapy combined with patient education improves quality of life of persons with obesity: a pilot feasibility study for a randomised controlled trial . Obes. Res. Clin. Pract. 11 , 79–87. 10.1016/j.orcp.2016.03.005 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Archer S., Buxton S., Sheffield D. (2015). The effect of creative psychological interventions on psychological outcomes for adult cancer patients: a systematic review of randomised controlled trials . Psychooncology 24 , 1–10. 10.1002/pon.3607 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • * . Aweto H. A., Owoeye O. B., Akinbo S. R., Onabajo A. A. (2012). Effects of dance movement therapy on selected cardiovascular parameters and estimated maximum oxygen consumption in hypertensive patients . Nig. Q. J. Hosp. Med. 22 , 125–129. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • * . Baptista A. S., Villela A. L., Jones A., Natour J. (2012). Effectiveness of dance in patients with fibromyalgia: a randomized, single-blind, controlled study . Clin. Exp. Rheumatol. 30 , 18–23. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Barba H., Knill M., Fuchs M. (1995). Minstrels of Soul: Intermodal Expressive Therapy . Toronto, ON: Palmerston Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Beelmann A., Heinrichs N. (2015). Evidence based psychotherapy, in International Encyclopedia of Social and Behavioral Sciences , 2nd Ed, Vol. 8. ed Wright J. D. (Oxford: Elsevier, 344–350. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Belardinelli R., Lacalaprice F., Ventrella C., Volpe L., Faccenda E. (2008). Waltz-dancing in patients with chronic heart failure: a new form of exercise training . Circ. Heart Fail. 1 , 107–114. 10.1161/CIRCHEARTFAILURE.108.765727 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Berrol C. F. (2000). The spectrum of research options in dance/movement therapy . Am. J. Dance Ther. 22 , 29–46. 10.1023/A:1005518409716 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • * . Biber K. (2016). The effects of folk dance training on 5-6 years children's physical and social development . J. Educ. Train. Stud. 4 , 213–226. 10.11114/jets.v4i11.1820 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Boehm K., Cramer H., Staroszynski T., Ostermann T. (2014). Arts therapies for anxiety, depression, and quality of life in breast cancer patients: a systematic review and meta-analysis . Evid. Based Complement. Altern. Med. 2014 :103297. 10.1155/2014/103297 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bohlmeijer E., Prenger R., Taal E., Cuijpers P. (2010). The effects of mindfulness-based stress reduction therapy on mental health of adults with a chronic medical disease: a meta-analysis . J. Psychosom. Res. 68 , 539–544. 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2009.10.005 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Borg W. R. (1993). Applying Educational Research: A Practical Guide . New York, NY: Longman. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bradt J., Goodill S. W., Dileo C. (2011). Dance/movement therapy for improving psychological and physical outcomes in cancer patients . Cochrane Database Syst. Rev . 1 :CD007103. 10.1002/14651858.CD007103.pub2 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bradt J., Shim M., Goodill S. W. (2015). Dance/movement therapy for improving psychological and physical outcomes in cancer patients . Cochrane Database Syst. Rev . 1 :CD007103. 10.1002/14651858.CD007103.pub3 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • * . Bräuninger I. (2012a). The efficacy of dance movement therapy group on improvement of quality of life: a randomized controlled trial . Arts Psychother. 39 , 296–303. 10.1016/j.aip.2012.03.008 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • * . Bräuninger I. (2012b). Dance movement therapy group intervention in stress treatment: a randomized controlled trial (RCT) . Arts Psychother. 39 , 443–450. 10.1016/j.aip.2012.07.002 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bräuninger I. (2014). Specific dance movement therapy interventions—which are successful? An intervention and correlation study . Arts Psychother. 41 , 445–457. 10.1016/j.aip.2014.08.002 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Burgess G., Grogan S., Burwitz L. (2006). Effects of a 6-week aerobic dance intervention on body image and physical self-perceptions in adolescent girls . Body Image 3 , 57–66. 10.1016/j.bodyim.2005.10.005 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Caldwell C. (1996). Getting Our Bodies Back . Boulder, CO: Shambala Edition. [ Google Scholar ]
  • * . Campion M., Levita L. (2014). Enhancing positive affect and divergent thinking abilities: play some music and dance . J. Posit. Psychol. 9 , 137–145. 10.1080/17439760.2013.848376 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • * . Chiang C. H., Chu C. L., Lee T. C. (2016). Efficacy of caregiver-mediated joint engagement intervention for young children with autism spectrum disorders . Autism 20 , 172–82. 10.1177/1362361315575725 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Chouhan S., Kumar S. (2011). Comparative study between effectiveness of dance movement therapy and progressive relaxation therapy with music for stress management in college students . Indian J. Physiother. Occup. Ther. 5 , 179–182. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Christensen J. F., Jola C. (2015). Moving towards ecological validity in empirical aesthetics of dance, in Art, Aesthetics and the Brain . eds Houston J. P., Nadal M., Mora F., Agnati L. F., Cela-Conde C. J. (Oxford: Oxford University Press; ), 223–262. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cohen J. (1988). The effect size, in Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioural Science , 2nd Edition. ed Erlbaum L. (New York, NY: Taylor and Francis Inc, 79–80. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Conceição L. S., Neto M. G., do Amaral M. A. S., Martins-Filho P. R. S., Oliveira Carvalho V. (2016). Effect of dance therapy on blood pressure and exercise capacity of individuals with hypertension: a systematic review and meta-analysis . Int. J. Cardiol. 220 , 553–557. 10.1016/j.ijcard.2016.06.182 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Connolly M. K., Quin E., Redding E. (2011). Dance 4 your life: exploring the health and well-being implications of a contemporary dance intervention for female adolescents . Res. Dance Educ. 12 , 53–66. 10.1080/14647893.2011.561306 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Coubard O. A., Duretz S., Lefebvre V., Lapalus P., Ferrufino L. (2011). Practice of contemporary dance improves cognitive flexibility in aging . Front. Aging Neurosci. 3 :13. 10.3389/fnagi.2011.00013 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cruz R. F., Berrol C. (2012). Dance/Movement Therapists in Action: A Working Guide to Research Options . Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cruz R. F., Sabers D. (1998). Dance/movement therapy is more effective than pre-previously reported . Arts Psychother. 25 , 101–104. [ Google Scholar ]
  • * . Cruz-Ferreira A., Marmeleira J., Formigo A., Gomes D., Fernandes J. (2015). Creative dance improves physical fitness and life satisfaction in older women . Res. Aging 37 , 837–855. 10.1177/0164027514568103 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Csikszentmihalyi M., Csikszentmihalyi I. (1975). Beyond Boredom and Anxiety . San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. [ Google Scholar ]
  • De Dreu M. J., van der Wilk A. S. D., Poppe E., Kwakkel G., van Wegen E. E. H. (2012). Rehabilitation, exercise therapy and music in patients with Parkinson's disease: a meta-analysis of the effects of music-based movement therapy on walking ability, balance and quality of life . Parkinsonism Relat. Disord. 18 ( Suppl. 1 ), S114–S119. 10.1016/S1353-8020(11)70036-0 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • * . De Natale E. R., Paulus K. S., Aiello E., Sanna B., Manca A., Sotgiu G., et al.. (2017). Dance therapy improves motor and cognitive functions in patients with Parkinson's disease . Neurorehabilitation 40 , 141–144. 10.3233/NRE-161399 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dreeben S. J., Mamberg M. H., Salmon P. (2013). The MBSR body scan in clinical practice . Mindfulness 4 , 394–401. 10.1007/s12671-013-0212-z [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • * . Duberg A., Hagberg L., Sunvisson H., Möller M. (2013). Influencing self-rated health among adolescent girls with dance intervention: a randomized controlled trial . JAMA Pediatr. 167 , 27–31. 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2013.421 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Duim E. L., do Nascimento C. F., da Silva A. (2015). Dance movement therapy as rehabilitation process for women with falls history . Gerontologist 55 :719 10.1093/geront/gnv358.03 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dunphy K., Baker F. A., Dumaresq E., Carroll-Haskins K., Eickholt J., Ercole M., et al.. (2019). Creative arts interventions to address depression in older adults: a systematic review of outcomes, processes, and mechanisms . Front. Psychol. 9 :2655. 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02655 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Duval S., Tweedie R. (2000). Trim and fill: a simple funnel-plot-based method of testing and adjusting for publication bias in meta-analysis . Biometrics 56 , 455–463. 10.1111/j.0006-341X.2000.00455.x [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • EADMT European Association Dance Movement Therapy (2018). Available online at: www.eadmt.com
  • Feniger-Schaal R., Hart Y., Lotan N., Koren-Karie N., Noy L. (2018). The body speaks: using the mirror game to link attachment and non-verbal behavior . Front. Psychol. 9 :1560. 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01560 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Fjorback L. O., Arendt M., Ørnbøl E., Fink P., Walach H. (2011). Mindfulness-based stress reduction and mindfulness-based cognitive therapy—a systematic review of randomized controlled trials . Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 124 , 102–119. 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2011.01704.x [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gomes Neto M., Menezes M. A., Carvalho V. O. (2014). Dance therapy in patients with chronic heart failure: a systematic review and a meta-analysis . Clin. Rehabil. 28 , 1172–1179. 10.1177/0269215514534089 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Goodill S. (2005). An Introduction to Medical Dance/Movement Therapy: Health Care in Motion . London; Philadelphia: Jessica Kingsley Publishers. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Grainge M. (2015). Excluding Small Studies From a Systematic Review or Metaanalysis . Available online at: https://skin.cochrane.org/sites/skin.cochrane.org/files/public/uploads/CSG-COUSIN_March%202015_M%20Grainge.pdf [accessed January 4, 2019].
  • Grawe K., Donati R., Bernauer F. (1994). Psychotherapie im Wandel: von der Konfession zur Profession . Göttingen: Verlag für Psychologie, Hogrefe. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Green S. B. (1991). How many subjects does it take to do a regression analysis? Multivariate Behav. Res . 26 , 499–510. 10.1207/s15327906mbr2603_7 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • * . Guerra-Balic M., Barnet-López S., Signo-Miguel S., Pérez-Testor S., Bruna O., Solano L., et al. (2017). Cognitive benefits of a dance movement therapy program in adults with intellectual disabilities . Med. Sci. Sport. Exerc. 49 :75 10.1249/01.mss.0000517033.34425.61 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Guzmán-García A., Hughes J. C., James I. A., Rochester L. (2013). Dancing as a psychosocial intervention in care homes: a systematic review of the literature . Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry . 28 , 914–924. 10.1002/gps.3913 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hackney M. E., Bennett C. G. (2014). Dance therapy for individuals with Parkinson's disease: improving quality of life . J. Park. Restless Legs Syndr. 4 , 17–25. 10.2147/JPRLS.S40042 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hall M. (2011). Dance/movement therapy in inpatient hematological cancer treatment in creative arts therapies (Theses) . Columbia College, Chicago, IL, United States, 5. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Harris R. J. (1985). A Primer of Multivariate Statistics ,2nd Edn. New York, NY: Academic Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • * . Hashimoto H., Takabatake S., Miyaguchi H., Nakanishi H., Naitou Y. (2015). Effects of dance on motor functions, cognitive functions, and mental symptoms of Parkinson's disease: a quasi-randomized pilot trial . Complement. Ther. Med. 23 , 210–219. 10.1016/j.ctim.2015.01.010 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hayes A. F. (2013). Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach . New York, NY: Guilford Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hedges L. V., Olkin I. (1985). Statistical Methods for Meta-Analysis . San Diego, CA: Academic Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hervey L. W. (2000). Artistic Inquiry in Dance/Movement Therapy: Creative Research Alternatives . Springfield, IL: Charles C Thomas Publisher. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Higgins J. P., Green S. (2008). Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions: Cochrane Book Series . West Sussex, UK: Wiley. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Higgins J. P., Thompson S. G., Deeks J. J., Altman D. G. (2003). Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses . BMJ 327 :557. 10.1136/bmj.327.7414.557 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Higgins J. P. T., Altman D. G., Gøtzsche P. C., Jüni P., Moher D., Oxman A. D., et al.. (2011). The Cochrane Collaboration's tool for assessing risk of bias in randomised trials . BMJ 343 :d5928. 10.1136/bmj.d5928 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • * . Hildebrandt M., Koch S., Fuchs T. (2016). We dance and find each other: effects of dance/movement therapy on negative symptoms in autism spectrum disorder . Behav. Sci. 6 :24 10.3390/bs6040024 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • * . Ho R. T. H., Fong T. C. T., Cheung I. K. M., Yip P. S. F., Luk M. Y. (2016a). Effects of a short-term dance movement therapy program on symptoms and stress in patients with breast cancer undergoing radiotherapy: a randomized, controlled, single-blind trial . J. Pain Symptom Manage. 51 , 824–831. 10.1016/j.jpainsymman.2015.12.332 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ho R. T. H., Lo P. H. Y., Luk M. Y. (2016b). A good time to dance? A mixed-methods approach of the effects of dance movement therapy for breast cancer patients during and after radiotherapy . Cancer Nurs. 39 , 32–41. 10.1097/NCC.0000000000000237 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hwang H. H., Choi Y. J., Kim B. R. (2010). The effects of dance therapy program on the cognitive function and memory of movement of elderly with dementia . Korean J. Phys. Educ. 49 , 373–382. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Jakobsen M., DeMott M. A. M., Wentzel-Larsen T., Heir T. (2017). The impact of the asylum process on mental health: a longitudinal study of unaccompanied refugee minors in Norway . BMJ Open 7 :e015157. 10.1136/bmjopen-2016-015157 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Jeong Y. J., Hong S. C., Myeong S. L., Park M. C., Kim Y. K., Suh C. M. (2005). Dance movement therapy improves emotional responses and modulates neurohormones in adolescents with mild depression . Int. J. Neurosci . 115 , 1711–1720. 10.1080/00207450590958574 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Jola C. (2013). Do you feel the same way too? in Touching and Being Touched: Kinesthesia and Empathy in Dance and Movement (Berlin: De Gruyter; ), 181–210. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Jola C., Calmeiro L. (2017). The dancing queen: explanatory mechanisms of the ‘feel-good effect’ in dance, in The Oxford Handbook of Dance and Wellbeing . eds Karkou V., Oliver S., Lykouris S. (London: Oxford University Press; ), 13–23. [ Google Scholar ]
  • * . Kaltsatou A., Kouidi E., Fountoulakis K., Sipka C., Theochari V., Kandylis D., et al.. (2015). Effects of exercise training with traditional dancing on functional capacity and quality of life in patients with schizophrenia: a randomized controlled study . Clin. Rehabil. 29 , 882–891. 10.1177/0269215514564085 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kaltsatou A., Mameletzi D., Douka S. (2011). Physical and psychological benefits of a 24-week traditional dance program in breast cancer survivors . J. Bodyw. Mov. Ther. 15 , 162–167. 10.1016/j.jbmt.2010.03.002 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Karkou V., Meekums B. (2017). Dance movement therapy for dementia . Cochrane Database Syst. Rev . CD011022. 10.1002/14651858.CD011022.pub2 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Karkou V., Sanderson P. (2006). Arts Therapies: A Research-Based Map of the Field . Amsterdam: Elsevier. [ Google Scholar ]
  • * . Kattenstroth J. C., Kalisch T., Holt S., Tegenthoff M., Dinse H. R. (2013). Six months of dance intervention enhances postural, sensorimotor, and cognitive performance in elderly without affecting cardio-respiratory functions . Front. Aging Neurosci. 5 :5. 10.3389/fnagi.2013.00005 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kazdin A. E. (2007). Mediators and mechanism of change in psychotherapy research . Ann. Rev. of Clin. Psych. 3 , 1–27. 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091432 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kiepe M. S., Stöckigt B., Keil T. (2012). Effects of dance therapy and ballroom dances on physical and mental illnesses: a systematic review . Arts Psychother. 39 , 404–411. 10.1016/j.aip.2012.06.001 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Koch S. C. (2011). Embodiment. Der Einfluss von Eigenbewegung auf Affekt, Einstellung und Kognition . Berlin: Logos. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Koch S. C. (2017). Arts and health: active factors and a theory framework of embodied aesthetics . Arts Psychother. 54 , 85–91. 10.1016/j.aip.2017.02.002 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Koch S. C. (2019). Indications and contraindications in dance movement therapy. An inductive approach from practitioners' experience . J. Arts Ther. 1. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Koch S. C., Kunz T., Lykou S., Cruz R. (2014). Effects of dance movement therapy and dance on health-related psychological outcomes: a meta-analysis . Arts Psychother. 41 , 46–64. 10.1016/j.aip.2013.10.004 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • * . Koch S. C., Mehl L., Sobanski E., Sieber M., Fuchs T. (2015a). Fixing the mirrors: a feasibility study of the effects of dance movement therapy on young adults with autism spectrum . Autism 19 , 338–350. 10.1177/1362361314522353 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Koch S. C., Morlinghaus K., Fuchs T. (2007). The joy dance: specific effects of a single dance intervention on psychiatric patients with depression . Arts Psychother. 34 , 340–349. 10.1016/j.aip.2007.07.001 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • * . Koch S. C., Ostermann T., Steinhage A., Kende P., Haller K., Chyle F. (2015b). Breaking barriers: evaluating an arts-based emotion regulation training in prison . Arts Psychother. 42 , 41–49. 10.1016/j.aip.2014.10.008 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • * . Koch S. C., Wirtz G., Harter C., Weisbrod M., Winkler F., Pröger A., et al. (2019). Embodied self in trauma and self-harm: effects of flamenco therapy on traumatized in-patients . A pilot study. J. Loss Trauma 24 , 441–459. 10.1080/15325024.2018.1507472 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • * . Koehne S., Behrends A., Fairhurst M. T., Dziobek I. (2016). Fostering social cognition through an imitation- and synchronization-based dance/movement intervention in adults with autism spectrum disorder: a controlled proof-of-concept study . Psychother. Psychosom. 85 , 27–35. 10.1159/000441111 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kraemer H. C., Thiemann S. (1987). How Many Subjects? Statistical Power Analysis in Research , 2nd Edn. London: Sage Publications. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lange G., Hartmann N., Eberhard-Kaechele M., Koch S. C. (in press). Arts-based research: aesthetic answering as a method of embodied analysis in the context of creative arts therapies, in Embodied Research Methods . ed Tantia J. (New York, NY: Routledge; ). [ Google Scholar ]
  • Leavy P. (2017). Handbook of Arts-Based Research. Guilford Publications . Available online at: https://books.google.de/books?id=TtpLMQAACAAJ [accessed January 4, 2019].
  • * . Lee H.-J., Jang S.-H., Lee S.-Y., Hwang K.-S. (2015). Effectiveness of dance/movement therapy on affect and psychotic symptoms in patients with schizophrenia . Arts Psychother. 45 , 64–68. 10.1016/j.aip.2015.07.003 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Levine B., Land H. M. (2016). A meta-synthesis of qualitative findings about dance/movement therapy for individuals with trauma . Qual. Health Res. 26 , 330–344. 10.1177/1049732315589920 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lipsey M. W., Wilson D. B. (2001). Practical Meta-Analysis . Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Loetzke D., Ostermann T., Büssing A. (2015). Argentine tango in Parkinson disease—a systematic review and meta-analysis . BMC Neurol. 15 :1 10.1186/s12883-015-0484-0 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Martin L., Oepen R., Bauer K., Nottensteiner A., Mergheim K., Gruber H., et al.. (2018). Creative arts interventions for stress management and prevention—a systematic review . Behav. Sci. 8 :2. 10.3390/bs8020028 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • * . Martin L. A. L., Koch S. C., Hirjak D., Fuchs T. (2016). Overcoming disembodiment: the effect of movement therapy on negative symptoms in schizophrenia—a multicenter randomized controlled trial . Front. Psychol. 7 :483. 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00483 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Maul A. (2013). Method effects and the meaning of measurement . Front. Psychol . 4 :169. 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00169 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • McCambridge J., Witton J., Elbourne D. R. (2014). Systematic review of the Hawthorne effect: new concepts are needed to study research participation effects . J. Clin. Epidemiol. 67 , 267–277. 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2013.08.015 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • McGarry L. M., Russo F. A. (2011). Mirroring in dance/movement therapy: potential mechanisms behind empathy enhancement . Arts Psychother. 38 , 178–184. 10.1016/j.aip.2011.04.005 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • McNiff S. (1993). The authority of experience . Arts Psychother. 20 , 3–9. 10.1016/0197-4556(93)90028-Z [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Meekums B., Karkou V., Nelson E. A. (2015). Dance movement therapy for depression . Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 39 , 126–133. 10.1002/14651858.CD009895.pub2 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • * . Meric O., Ilhan A. (2016). Does 12-week Latin dance training affect the self-confidence of the university students? J. Educ. Learn. 5 , 159–164. 10.5539/jel.v5n4p159 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Michalak J., Heidenreich T., Meibert P., Schulte D. J. (2008). Mindfulness predicts relapse/recurrence in major depressive disorder after mindfulness-based cognitive therapy . Nerv. Ment. Dis. 196 , 630–633. 10.1097/NMD.0b013e31817d0546 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Murrock C. J., Madigan E. (2008). Self-efficacy and social support as mediators between culturally specific dance and lifestyle physical activity . Res. Theory Nurs. Pract. 22 , 192–204. 10.1891/1541-6577.22.3.192 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Palo-Bengtsson L., Ekman S. L. (1997). Social dancing in the care of persons with dementia in a nursing home setting: a phenomenological study . Sch. Inq. Nurs. Pract. 11 , 101–118. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Palo-Bengtsson L., Winblad B., Ekman S. L. (1998). Social dancing: a way to support intellectual, emotional and motor functions in persons with dementia . J. Psychiatr. Ment. Health Nurs . 5 , 545–554. 10.1046/j.1365-2850.1998.560545.x [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • * . Panagiotopoulou E. (2018). Dance therapy and the public school: the development of social and emotional skills of high school students in Greece . Arts Psychother. 59 , 25–33. 10.1016/j.aip.2017.11.003 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Peters H. J. (2012). Dance/dance movement therapy and general wellbeing, depression, and anxiety: A meta-analysis (Doctoral dissertation: ). University of Tasmania. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pilates J. H., Miller W. J. (1945). Return to Life Through Contrology (Reprint 1998) . New York, NY: Presentation Dynamics Inc. [ Google Scholar ]
  • * . Pinniger R., Brown R. F., Thorsteinsson E. B., McKinley P. (2012). Argentine tango dance compared to mindfulness meditation and a waiting-list control: a randomised trial for treating depression . Complement. Ther. Med. 20 , 377–384. 10.1016/j.ctim.2012.07.003 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • * . Pinniger R., Thorsteinsson E. B., Brown R. F., McKinley P. (2013). Tango dance can reduce distress and insomnia in people with self-referred affective symptoms . Am. J. Dance Ther. 35 , 60–77. 10.1007/s10465-012-9141-y [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pohlmann V., Koch S. C., Fuchs T. (2017). Changes in well-being of schizophrenic patients after movement therapy: results of a multicenter RCT study, in Arts Therapies and New Challenges in Psychiatry (London: Routledge; ), 114–139. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Price C., Smith-DiJulio K. (2016). Interoceptive awareness is important for relapse prevention: perceptions of women who received mindful body-awareness in substance use disorder treatment . J. Addict. Nurs. 27 , 32–38. 10.1097/JAN.0000000000000109 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • * . Priebe S., Savill M., Wykes T., Bentall R. P., Reininghaus U., Lauber C., et al.. (2016). Effectiveness of group body psychotherapy for negative symptoms of schizophrenia: multicentre randomised controlled trial . Br. J. Psychiatry . 209 , 54–61. 10.1192/bjp.bp.115.171397 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • * . Pylvänäinen P. M., Muotka J. S., Lappalainen R. (2015). A dance movement therapy group for depressed adult patients in a psychiatric outpatient clinic: effects of the treatment . Front. Psychol. 6 :980. 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00980 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Quiroga Murcia C., Bongard S., Kreutz G. (2009). Emotional and neurohumoral responses to dancing tango argentino: the effects of music and partner . Music Med. 1 , 14–21. 10.1177/1943862109335064 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ree M. J., Craigie M. A. (2007). Outcomes following mindfulness-based cognitive therapy in a heterogeneous sample of adult outpatients . Behav. Change . 24 , 70–86. 10.1375/bech.24.2.70 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • * . Rios Romenets S., Anang J., Fereshtehnejad S. M., Pelletier A., Postuma R. (2015). Tango for treatment of motor and non-motor manifestations in Parkinson's disease: a randomized control study . Complement. Ther. Med. 23 , 175–184. 10.1016/j.ctim.2015.01.015 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ritter M., Graff Low K. (1996). Effects of dance/movement therapy: a meta-analysis . Arts Psychother. 23 , 249–260. 10.1016/0197-4556(96)00027-5 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rodrigues-Krause J., Farinha J. B., Krause M., Reischak-Oliveira Á. (2016). Effects of dance interventions on cardiovascular risk with ageing: systematic review and meta-analysis . Complement. Ther. Med. 29 , 16–28. 10.1016/j.ctim.2016.09.004 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Roethlisberger F. J., Dickson W. J. (1964). Management and the Worker: An Account of a Research Program Conducted by the Western Electric Company, Hawthorne Works, Chicago, by FJ Roethlisberger and William J. Dickson, With the Assistance and Collaboration of Harold A. Wright . Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Röhricht F., Papadopoulos N., Priebe S. (2013). An exploratory randomized controlled trial of body psychotherapy for patients with chronic depression . J. Affect. Disord. 151 , 85–91. 10.1016/j.jad.2013.05.056 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • * . Röhricht F., Priebe S. (2006). Effect of body-oriented psychological therapy on negative symptoms in schizophrenia: a randomized controlled trial . Psychol. Med. 36 , 669–678. 10.1017/S0033291706007161 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rosenthal R. (1966). Experimenter Effects in Behavioral Research . East Norwalk, CT: Appleton-Century-Crofts. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rosenthal R. (1979). The file drawer problem and tolerance for null results . Psychol. Bull . 86 , 638–641. 10.1037/0033-2909.86.3.638 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rutan J. S., Stone W. N. (2001). Psychodynamic Group Psychotherapy . 3rd Edn. New York, NY: Guilford Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sackett D. L., Straus S. E., Richardson S. W., Rosenberg W., Haynes B. R. (2000). Evidence-Based Medicine: How to Practice and Teach EBM . London: Churchill Livingstone. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sandel S. L., Chaiklin S., Lohn A. (1993). Foundations of Dance Movement Therapy: The Life and Work of Marian Chace. Marian Chace Memorial Fund of the American Dance Therapy Association. Columbia: American Dance Therapy Association. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schmais C. (1985). Healing processes in group dance therapy . Am. J. Dance Ther . 8 , 17–36. 10.1007/BF02251439 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schmais C. (1998). Understanding the dance/movement therapy group . Am. J. Dance Ther. 20 , 23–35. 10.1023/A:1022845708143 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • * . Serrano-Guzmán M., Valenza-Peña C. M., Serrano-Guzmán C., Aguilar-Ferrándiz E., Valenza-Demet G., Villaverde-Gutiérrez C. (2016). Effects of a dance therapy programme on quality of life, sleep and blood pressure in middle-aged women: a randomised controlled trial . Med. Clín. 147 , 334–339. 10.1016/j.medcle.2016.11.016 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Shanahan J., Morris M. E., Bhriain O. N., Saunders J., Clifford A. M. (2015). Dance for people with Parkinson's disease: what is the evidence telling us? Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil . 96 , 141–153. 10.1016/j.apmr.2014.08.017 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sharp K., Hewitt J. (2014). Dance as an intervention for people with Parkinson's disease: a systematic review and meta-analysis . Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev . 47 , 445–456. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.09.009 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sharpe D. (1997). Of apples and oranges, file drawers and garbage: why validity issues in meta-analysis will not go away . Clin. Psychol. Rev. 17 , 881–901. 10.1016/S0272-7358(97)00056-1 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Stück M., Villegas A. (2017). Evidence-based BIODANZA programmes for children (TANZPRO-Biodanza) in schools and kinder-gartens, in The Oxford Handbook of Dance and Wellbeing , 75–96. [ Google Scholar ]
  • * . Sturm I., Baak J., Storek B., Traore A., Thuss-Patience P. (2014). Effect of dance on cancer-related fatigue and quality of life . Support. Care Cancer 22 , 2241–2249. 10.1007/s00520-014-2181-8 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Teasdale J. D., Segal Z. V., Williams J. M., Ridgeway V. A., Soulsby J. M., Lau M. A. (2000). Prevention of relapse/recurrence in major depression by mindfulness-based cognitive therapy . J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 68 , 615–623. 10.1037/0022-006X.68.4.615 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • * . Teixeira-Machado L., Azevedo-Santos I., De Santana J. M. (2017). Dance improves functionality and psychosocial adjustment in cerebral palsy: a randomized controlled clinical trial . Am. J. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 96 , 424–429. 10.1097/PHM.0000000000000646 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Van Westrhenen N., Fritz E., Vermeer A., Boelen P., Kleber R. (2019). Creative arts in psychotherapy for traumatized children in South Africa: an evaluation study . PLoS ONE 14 :e0210857. 10.1371/journal.pone.0210857 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • * . Vankova H., Holmerova I., Machacova K., Volicer L., Veleta P., Celko A. M. (2014). The effect of dance on depressive symptoms in nursing home residents . J. Am. Med. Dir. Assoc. 15 , 582–587. 10.1016/j.jamda.2014.04.013 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • * . Ventura M. I., Barnes D. E., Ross J. M., Lanni K. E., Sigvardt K. A., Disbrow E. A. (2016). A pilot study to evaluate multi-dimensional effects of dance for people with Parkinson's disease . Contemp. Clin. Trials 51 , 50–55. 10.1016/j.cct.2016.10.001 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • * . Volpe D., Signorini M., Marchetto A., Lynch T., Morris M. E. (2013). A comparison of Irish set dancing and exercises for people with Parkinson's disease: a phase II feasibility study . BMC Geriatr. 13 :54. 10.1186/1471-2318-13-54 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wampold B. E. (2015). How important are the common factors in psychotherapy? An update. World Psychiatry 14 , 270–277. 10.1002/wps.20238 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wampold B. E., Irmel Z. E. (2015). The Great Psychotherapy Debate: The Evidence for What Makes Psychotherapy Work . 2nd EdN. New York, NY: Routledge. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wengrower H., Chaiklin S. (2008). The Art and Science of Dance/Movement Therapy: Life is Dance . New York, NY: Routledge. [ Google Scholar ]
  • * . Wiedenhofer S., Koch S. C. (2017). Active factors in dance/movement therapy: specifying health effects of non-goal-orientation in movement . Arts Psychother. 52 , 10–23. 10.1016/j.aip.2016.09.004 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wilson D. B. (2005). Meta-Analysis Macros for SAS, SPSS, and Stata . Available online at: https://mason.gmu.edu/%7Edwilsonb/ma.html [accessed January 4, 2019].
  • Xiong L.i M., Li Q. (2009). Influence of dance therapy of self-efficacy and rehabilitation of patients with depression . Chin. Nurs. Res. 23 , 3138–3139. 10.3969/j.issn.1009-6493.2009.34.015 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Yalom I. D. (1985). The Theory and Practice of Group Psychotherapy . 3rd Edn. New York, NY: Basic Books. [ Google Scholar ]

Choreographic Analysis as Dance Studies Methodology

  • In book: Dance Research Methodologies (pp.113-133)
  • Publisher: Routledge
  • This person is not on ResearchGate, or hasn't claimed this research yet.

J. Lorenzo Perillo at University of Hawaiʻi at Mānoa

  • University of Hawaiʻi at Mānoa

Discover the world's research

  • 25+ million members
  • 160+ million publication pages
  • 2.3+ billion citations

Gregory Alan Babbitt

  • Miranda L. Lynch

Ernest Fokoue

  • Sophie Pui Sim Mo
  • Renato Noguera

J. Lorenzo Perillo

  • Mara Mandradjieff
  • Francisca Reyes Aquino

Reyson Candelario

  • Randy Martin

Cristina Fernandes Rosa

  • Jonathan David Jackson
  • Thomas F. DeFrantz
  • Recruit researchers
  • Join for free
  • Login Email Tip: Most researchers use their institutional email address as their ResearchGate login Password Forgot password? Keep me logged in Log in or Continue with Google Welcome back! Please log in. Email · Hint Tip: Most researchers use their institutional email address as their ResearchGate login Password Forgot password? Keep me logged in Log in or Continue with Google No account? Sign up
  • DOI: 10.1117/12.2667704
  • Corpus ID: 257845917

Design and implementation of art grading line dance scoring system based on Android and SQLite

  • Ting Meng , Han Li
  • Published in Fifth International… 28 March 2023
  • Computer Science

7 References

Related papers.

Showing 1 through 3 of 0 Related Papers

Who Is AI Replacing? The Impact of Generative AI on Online Freelancing Platforms

31 Pages Posted: 3 Nov 2023 Last revised: 22 Jun 2024

Ozge Demirci

Harvard business school, jonas hannane.

German Institute for Economic Research (DIW Berlin); Technische Universität Berlin (TU Berlin)

Xinrong Zhu

Imperial College Business School

Date Written: October 15, 2023

This paper studies the impact of Generative AI technologies on the demand for online freelancers using a large dataset from a leading global freelancing platform. We identify the types of jobs that are more affected by Generative AI and quantify the magnitude of the heterogeneous impact. Our findings indicate a 21% decrease in the number of job posts for automation-prone jobs related to writing and coding, compared to jobs requiring manual-intensive skills, within eight months after the introduction of Chat-GPT. We show that the reduction in the number of job posts increases competition among freelancers while the remaining automation-prone jobs are of greater complexity and offer higher pay. We also find that the introduction of Image-generating AI technologies led to a 17% decrease in the number of job posts related to image creation. We use Google Trends to show that the more pronounced decline in the demand for freelancers within automation-prone jobs correlates with their higher public awareness of ChatGPT's substitutability.

Keywords: Generative AI, large language models, ChatGPT, digital freelancing platforms JEL No: O33, E24, J21, J24

JEL Classification: O33, E24, J21, J24

Suggested Citation: Suggested Citation

Harvard University Harvard Business School, 25 Harvard Way Boston, MA Boston 02163 United States

German Institute for Economic Research (DIW Berlin) ( email )

Mohrenstraße 58 Berlin, 10117 Germany

Technische Universität Berlin (TU Berlin) ( email )

Straße des 17 Juni 135 Berlin, 10623 Germany

Xinrong Zhu (Contact Author)

Imperial college business school ( email ).

South Kensington Campus Exhibition Road London SW7 2AZ, SW7 2AZ United Kingdom 07778963203 (Phone)

Do you have a job opening that you would like to promote on SSRN?

Paper statistics, related ejournals, ebusiness & ecommerce ejournal.

Subscribe to this fee journal for more curated articles on this topic

Decision-Making & Management Science eJournal

International political economy: globalization ejournal, decision-making in computational design & technology ejournal.

line dance research paper

  • Cast & crew

Nicole Kidman in Babygirl (2024)

A high-powered CEO puts her career and family on the line when she begins a torrid affair with her much younger intern. A high-powered CEO puts her career and family on the line when she begins a torrid affair with her much younger intern. A high-powered CEO puts her career and family on the line when she begins a torrid affair with her much younger intern.

  • Halina Reijn
  • Nicole Kidman
  • Harris Dickinson
  • Antonio Banderas

Babygirl (2024)

  • Intern Rose

Maxwell Whittington-Cooper

  • Uber Driver

Alex Anagnostidis

  • All cast & crew
  • Production, box office & more at IMDbPro

More like this

Margo's Got Money Troubles

  • December 20, 2024 (United States)
  • United States
  • New York City, New York, USA (street scenes)
  • Man Up Film
  • See more company credits at IMDbPro

Technical specs

Related news, contribute to this page.

Nicole Kidman in Babygirl (2024)

  • See more gaps
  • Learn more about contributing

More to explore

Recently viewed.

line dance research paper

Advertisement

A Visual Timeline of the Trump Rally Shooting

By Leanne Abraham ,  June Kim ,  Elena Shao ,  Julie Walton Shaver ,  Anjali Singhvi ,  Christiaan Triebert ,  Karen Yourish ,  Lazaro Gamio and Amy Schoenfeld Walker

The Associated Press; Photograph by Doug Mills/The New York Times

Former President Donald J. Trump was whisked off the stage at his rally in Pennsylvania on Saturday after gunshots were fired toward the area where he was speaking. Mr. Trump could be seen bleeding from his right ear, and officials said that the shooting was being investigated as an assassination attempt.

Site of Trump rally the morning after shots were fired

Location of law

enforcement

snipers on roof

Location of gunman’s body

Location of a

wounded rally

Stage where

Trump was speaking

Butler Farm

Show Grounds

Pennsylvania

Location of

gunman’s body

By The New York Times; Aerial image by Doral Chenoweth/USA TODAY NETWORK, via Reuters

The rally took place on the grounds of the Butler Farm Show in western Pennsylvania, about 35 miles north of Pittsburgh. On Saturday afternoon, tens of thousands of rally attendees started trickling in after the doors opened at 1 p.m.

Mr. Trump was set to begin speaking at 5 p.m., but didn’t appear onstage until about an hour later. Here’s how the next 11 minutes unfolded based on footage of the rally.

6:05 p.m.   As the song concludes, Mr. Trump approaches the lectern and begins speaking. He spends the first six minutes talking about President Biden and the state of the country before focusing on immigration.

line dance research paper

Eric Lee/The New York Times

TMX via Associated Press

Seconds later   A gunshot is heard, and Mr. Trump stops midsentence and flinches. He reaches for his right ear, as another two shots are quickly fired, and ducks behind the lectern. One male Secret Service agent is heard yelling, “Get down, get down, get down, get down!”

Photo appears to

capture the path

of a projectile

Blood on Trump’s

hand after he

touches his ear

Photo appears

to capture the

path of a projectile

Photos by Doug Mills/The New York Times

Secret Service agents surround Mr. Trump as a burst of five more shots is fired. Members of the crowd are panicking, screaming and crouching down. More security personnel run onto the stage, including several heavily armed law enforcement agents.

About 42 seconds after shooting began   Agents stay crouched over Mr. Trump until an agent can be heard saying, “Shooter down.” The Secret Service confirmed later in a statement that its “personnel neutralized the shooter.”

line dance research paper

Doug Mills/The New York Times

line dance research paper

A spectator who had been standing just outside the grounds said in an interview with the BBC that a few minutes into Mr. Trump’s speech, he noticed that someone was “bear-crawling up the roof,” clearly armed with a rifle, and that he tried to notify the police. Law enforcement officials later said that the gunman had opened fire from an elevated position outside the rally’s security perimeter.

After the shooting, the gunman’s body was seen on the rooftop of one of the buildings to Mr. Trump’s right. An AR-15-style semiautomatic rifle was recovered at the scene, according to law enforcement officials.

“I was shot with a bullet that pierced the upper part of my right ear,” Mr. Trump said in a post on Truth Social, his social media platform. “I knew immediately that something was wrong in that I heard a whizzing sound, shots, and immediately felt the bullet ripping through the skin.”

Shortly after the shooting, a spokesman for Mr. Trump’s campaign said that the former president was “fine.” Mr. Trump will still attend the Republican National Convention this week, his advisers said in a statement.

One spectator at the rally, Corey Comperatore , a 50-year-old firefighter, was shot and killed. Two others were critically injured.

Scenes from after the shooting

Secret Service escorts Trump offstage.

Photographs by Eric Lee and Doug Mills/The New York Times

  • Share full article

IMAGES

  1. Dance Research Paper Free Essay Example

    line dance research paper

  2. Idaw Dance: Research-Based Insights Free Essay Example

    line dance research paper

  3. Modern Dance

    line dance research paper

  4. Passion And Salsa Dance Essay Example

    line dance research paper

  5. Dance Research and Presentation Assignment by Marie Modica

    line dance research paper

  6. Dance research paper ideas in 2021

    line dance research paper

VIDEO

  1. Mendocino

  2. Line Dance Basics

  3. Rolling down Under

  4. Players

  5. Levitating

  6. Margareasons

COMMENTS

  1. Education Sciences

    Line dancing has proven to be an effective physical activity for improving the state of depression in late second- and third-age dancers. ... Feature papers represent the most advanced research with significant potential for high impact in the field. A Feature Paper should be a substantial original Article that involves several techniques or ...

  2. Effects of line dancing on physical function and perceived limitation

    Line dancing is a popular form of exercise that can be modified, and is thus feasible for older adults with mobility limitations. Purpose: The purpose of this study was to assess the effects of 8 weeks of line dancing on balance, muscle strength, lower extremity function, endurance, gait speed, and perceived mobility limitations.

  3. (PDF) Effects of line dancing on physical function and perceived

    The 8 week line dancing intervention was a low impact dance program (i.e., one foot was always in contact with the floor), previ- ously considered appropriate for older adults with mobility limita-

  4. Dancing and the Brain

    Music stimulates the brain's reward centers, while dance activates its sensory and motor circuits. Studies using PET imaging have identified regions of the brain that contribute to dance learning and performance. These regions include the motor cortex, somatosensory cortex, basal ganglia, and cerebellum. The motor cortex is involved in the ...

  5. University of Northern Colorado

    University of Northern Colorado

  6. Dance Research Journal

    Dance Research Journal is the longest running, peer reviewed journal in its field, and has become one of the foremost international outlets for dance research scholarship. The journal carries scholarly articles, book reviews, and a list of books and journals received. ... London: Routledge. 358 pp., 30 b/w illustrations. $37.56 paper and e-book ...

  7. Line Dance Impact on Female Students' Physical Condition

    Li Shi Teachers Campus girls' 18-week line dance training is to test the change of physical indicators before and after learning.Based on that,the impact of line dancing exercise on physical health is analyzed to further investigate fitness value of line dancing exercise.The results show that line dancing improves student body shape,function and coordination.

  8. PDF An Empirical Study on Line Dancing to Promote the Construction of

    An Empirical Study on Line Dancing to Promote the Construction of Community Sports Culture . Yu Yan . Xianyang Normal University Shaanxi Xianyang, 712000, China . [email protected] . ... and provides theoretical and methodological basis for the research of this paper. In order to make the content of the paper more effective, I interviewed 15 ...

  9. Line Dance and Its Aesthetic Value Analysis

    Line dance is a dance,and also one kind of movement,even a kind of culture.Line dance can not only exercise,but also improve people's artistic accomplishment.In this paper,the classification,characteristics and style are summarized.From the musical beauty,body beauty and beauty of body action,exhibit beauty and beauty of dress and so on,the dance aesthetic value is analyzed.The result shows ...

  10. Research on How to Strengthen College Students' Mastering of Skill

    However, in fact line dance can effectively strengthen college students' physical function, cultivate their music literacy and enhance their psychological quality. Therefore, this paper further strengthens college students' line dance skills through the study of these three elements. 2. Research Objects and Methods.

  11. An Empirical Study on Line Dancing to Promote the Construction of

    This paper takes the community sports of a city as the research object and the characteristics of line dance as the axis to discuss the reasons why line dance enters the community and the development countermeasures, so as to provide theoretical basis and practical reference for the sustainable and healthy development of line dance in community ...

  12. The Physiological and Psychological Benefits of Dance and its Effects

    For example, participants can select the intensity levels, dance routines/mode, dance music, even dance game partners. In a research study investigating 36 h of dance exergaming lasting 12 weeks, researchers observed a decrease in adiposity and an increase in bone mineral density compared to a non-exercising control group (Staiano et al., 2017b ...

  13. (PDF) Moving Object Detection Technology of Line Dancing Based on

    In this paper, line dancing's moving object detection technology based on machine vision is studied to improve object detection. For this purpose, the improved frame difference for the ...

  14. Effects of Dance Movement Therapy and Dance on Health-Related

    Background: Dance is an embodied activity and, when applied therapeutically, can have several specific and unspecific health benefits. In this meta-analysis, we evaluated the effectiveness of dance movement therapy 1 (DMT) and dance interventions for psychological health outcomes. Research in this area grew considerably from 1.3 detected studies/year in 1996-2012 to 6.8 detected studies/year ...

  15. Dance Research: The Journal of the Society for Dance Research

    Dance Research is edited and published in Britain with the assistance of a distinguished group of editorial consultants based in Europe and the USA. The journal provides an international forum for the presentation and discussion of contemporary dance research and contains a section of comprehensive book and journal reviews. Journal information.

  16. Thinking Strategically about Dance Making: An Analysis ...

    Events: The Society holds regular events for the presentation of the findings of dance research and for their discussion. These include the presentation of papers, lectures, demonstrations and research seminars. National Film Archives: Dance film and video from the BBC archives has been made available by a joint project funded by the Society.

  17. (PDF) Choreographic Analysis as Dance Studies Methodology

    Abstract. This chapter stages a conversation about the development of choreographic analysis as a methodology, since Susan Leigh Foster's 1986 book, Reading Dancing. Through her analyses, Foster ...

  18. Researching Dance: Evolving Modes of Inquiry on JSTOR

    InResearching Dance, an introduction to research methods in dance addressed primarily to graduate students, the editors explore dance as evolutional, defining i...

  19. Dancing on the inside: A qualitative study on online dance learning

    In this paper, we propose a new attempt toward online learning, especially for dancing, which can provide timely and substantial feedback for dance novices to improve learning experiences. We design and develop DancingInside , an online dance learning system that supports dance learning through a 'practice, feedback, reflection, and ...

  20. Dance Research: Vol 41, No 2

    Dance Research, the journal of the Society for Dance Research, is a bi-annual internationally peer reviewed journal.It welcomes high quality original research articles on dance worldwide both historical and contemporary. The journal aims to engage with current debates on dance and across cognate disciplines with dance at the centre of inquiry.

  21. Dance Research

    Dance Research, the journal of the Society for Dance Research, is a bi-annual internationally peer reviewed journal.It welcomes high quality original research articles on dance worldwide both historical and contemporary. The journal aims to engage with current debates on dance and across cognate disciplines with dance at the centre of inquiry.

  22. Design and implementation of art grading line dance scoring system

    The implementation of the system effectively improves the efficiency of scoring in the line dance competitions and ensures the correctness of the final score. Combined with the actual needs of scoring in line dancing competition, an Android-based competition scoring system is designed. The system is mainly divided into Android terminal and PC terminal. The administrator can enter the main ...

  23. 'You Think, So You Can Dance?' Science Is on It

    David Leventhal, a former dancer with the Mark Morris Dance Group who is the program director and a founding teacher of Dance for PD, has been a co-author on several recent scientific papers about ...

  24. PDF Mahesh Dattani's Dance like a Man: A Study of Gender Discrimination

    RESEARCH PAPER Mahesh Dattani's Dance like a Man: A Study of Gender Discrimination *Dr. Vandana Bhartiya Ph.D. from Department of English & MEL, University of Allahabad, Allahabad ... The line shows the notions of the society that demands man should behave like a man. It shows that man cannot even expect to wear a dress of a woman.

  25. Photo Appears to Capture Path of Bullet Used in Assassination Attempt

    Michael Harrigan, a retired F.B.I. special agent, said the image captured by Doug Mills, a New York Times photographer, seems to show a bullet streaking past former President Donald J. Trump.

  26. PDF BEYOND BYTES AND BEATS: The Preservation of Digital Live performance Art

    This thesis, presented as a research paper, seeks to explore the preservation of two main categories: live coding performances and live mixed-media performances. The first chapter will focus on defining digital live performance art (DLPA) and establishing the parameters of this form of artwork. It will then introduce the two aforementioned

  27. Who Is AI Replacing? The Impact of Generative AI on Online ...

    This paper studies the impact of Generative AI technologies on the demand for online freelancers using a large dataset from a leading global freelancing platform. We identify the types of jobs that are more affected by Generative AI and quantify the magnitude of the heterogeneous impact.

  28. What We Know About the Global Microsoft Outage

    Across the world, critical businesses and services including airlines, hospitals, train networks and TV stations, were disrupted on Friday by a global tech outage affecting Microsoft users.

  29. Babygirl (2024)

    Babygirl: Directed by Halina Reijn. With Nicole Kidman, Harris Dickinson, Antonio Banderas, Jean Reno. A high-powered CEO puts her career and family on the line when she begins a torrid affair with her much younger intern.

  30. A Visual Timeline of the Trump Rally Shooting

    Shots rang out as former President Donald J. Trump spoke at a campaign event in Butler, Pa., on Saturday. Maps, photos and video show how the shooting unfolded.