U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings

Preview improvements coming to the PMC website in October 2024. Learn More or Try it out now .

  • Advanced Search
  • Journal List
  • PMC10142271

Logo of jintell

Language: Its Origin and Ongoing Evolution

Ilia markov.

1 Department of Psychology, University of Houston, Houston, TX 77204, USA

2 Texas Institute for Measurement, Evaluation, and Statistics (TIMES), The University of Houston, Houston, TX 77204, USA

3 Center for Cognitive Sciences, Sirius University for Science and Technology, Sochi 354340, Russia

Kseniia Kharitonova

Elena l. grigorenko.

4 Baylor College of Medicine, Houston, TX 77030, USA

5 Child Study Center and Haskins Laboratories, Yale University, New Haven, CT 06520, USA

6 Rector’s Office, Moscow State University for Psychology and Education, Moscow 127051, Russia

Associated Data

Not applicable.

With the present paper, we sought to use research findings to illustrate the following thesis: the evolution of language follows the principles of human evolution. We argued that language does not exist for its own sake, it is one of a multitude of skills that developed to achieve a shared communicative goal, and all its features are reflective of this. Ongoing emerging language adaptations strive to better fit the present state of the human species. Theories of language have evolved from a single-modality to multimodal, from human-specific to usage-based and goal-driven. We proposed that language should be viewed as a multitude of communication techniques that have developed and are developing in response to selective pressure. The precise nature of language is shaped by the needs of the species (arguably, uniquely H. sapiens ) utilizing it, and the emergence of new situational adaptations, as well as new forms and types of human language, demonstrates that language includes an act driven by a communicative goal. This article serves as an overview of the current state of psycholinguistic research on the topic of language evolution.

1. Introduction

Research on language origin and evolution may be viewed as two of the most prominent research directions of the past few years. While these questions have been at the forefront of language science since its inception, only recently have we seen methodologies and techniques being developed that can provide answers backed with sufficient empirical evidence. The landscape of theoretical frameworks of language origin, the form in which it originated, and its worldwide dispersal has also been shifting in response to newly obtained evidence. The field of language evolution research can be described as currently coming of age while already equipped with a rich toolkit of methods for pursuits such as comparative research, investigating commonalities and differences between human language and animal communication systems, and studying cumulative cultural evolution of communication systems in experimental settings ( Dediu and de Boer 2016 ).

The aim of this article was to give a brief overview of the evolution of language, as well as to demonstrate how theoretical frameworks of language origin and evolution evolved with it. We sought to utilize the latest findings to argue that evolution is one of the central driving forces of the existence and development of human language. Language is a human skill, the nature and features of which are shaped in accordance with the needs of the species through continuous usage and adherence to communicative goals ( Grigorenko 2023 ). That nature is reflected through newly emergent language origin theories that move away from the innateness of language and provide plausible explanations of gradual language emergence from a multitude of other subsystems of communication. In regard to language modality, the main informational channel of origin, we intended to provide theoretical reasoning and evidence for the multimodal approach.

To illustrate that language is a skill that constantly undergoes changes due to various selective pressures, we aimed to explore three groups of factors that seem to transform language in the most significant ways: factors of the physical environment (such as aridity, vegetation, ambient temperatures, precipitation, latitude), socio-demographic factors (number of language users, geographic spread, degree of language contact, and the role of communicative situations), and technological advances (Internet, smartphones, and instant messaging). The latter group of factors is of most interest to us due to the changes that online communication is bringing about at a rate that has never been witnessed before. We propose that the evolution of language follows a similar pattern to the one outlined by recent research into general intelligence, promoting a more context-dependent and dynamic view of intelligence that is focused more radically than ever before on niche construction as a result of the cultural evolution ( Preiss 2022 ). The factors of the environment that influence language now reflect the changes brought along with the Anthropocene ( Anthropocene Working Group 2019 ), which further alters the ecological niche of our species, necessitating further adaptation.

While the argumentation provided in the present article is split into two main sections, covering the theories of origin and the evidence of evolution, all of it follows the main thesis of language emerging and evolving through usage; we deem this thesis to be the most prominent new direction in language research.

2. Origin of Language

There are more than 7000 living languages across the globe today ( Lewis 2009 ). To approach the topic of the ongoing evolution of language that spans millennia, we first need to determine its origin. However, the question by itself poses a challenge.

An important philosophical distinction collects two separate topics under the label “origin of language”: the origin of language faculty and the origin of languages ( Formigari 2013 ). The latter leads to a further question, namely, whether all world languages derive from one single “protolanguage”—i.e., monogenesis—or do several language families (e.g., Indo-European, Afro-Asiatic, Altaic) each derive from a different protolanguage—i.e., polygenesis ( Graffi 2019 ).

The theories relating to the emergence of language have been extensively debated for centuries, once leading to the infamous ban of the germane discussions by the Société de Linguistique de Paris in 1861. However, the field has seen a surge in this theorizing since the late 20th century and its considerable evolution over the past two decades ( Nölle et al. 2020b ). The focus of the debate has been on the innateness of language for many years. The central line of research that incorporates the traditional generative view ( Chomsky 1988 ), notions of innateness, universal grammar (UG), and the poverty of stimulus argument is largely classified as biolinguistics (for a more modern iteration of this framework, see ( Boeckx 2021 ; Bolhuis et al. 2014 ; Hauser et al. 2002 )). Such approaches argue that knowledge of language structures is impossible to extract from linguistic input; hence, it is suggested to be innate. An opposing point of view, referred to as “usage-based” or “emergentist” approaches, postulates that language emerged from its usage: “meaning is use—structure emerges from use” ( Tomasello 2009, p. 69 ); linguistic knowledge in these approaches proceeds via the abstraction and schematization of actual language use into fixed chunks, as well as more abstract linguistic patterns that become cognitively entrenched. These approaches, therefore, reject the notion of UG ( Pleyer and Hartmann 2019 ), instead utilizing the notion of the common communicative goal to explain language commonalities ( Arbib 2012 ). Notably, attempts to synthesize the opposing theories were few and far between. Such an attempt was undertaken by Pinker and Bloom ( 1990 ), where the authors put forth a compelling case against viewing language as a “spandrel”—an architectural allegory of a space formed at the intersection of other spaces, its shape therefore not being a significant trait on its own—arguing that certain constraints would not allow us to assume a non-adaptationist point of view, as “no adaptive organ can be adaptive in every aspect” (1990, p. 19).

In these controversies, one of the most contested topics is the question of causality, which is influenced by several factors: the problem of spurious correlation ( Roberts and Winters 2012 ), the universally concerning “replication crisis” ( Open Science Collaboration 2015 ), and a tendency to rely on indirect evidence ( Nölle et al. 2020b ). A novel solution for this contest is termed the “maximum robustness approach,” which, instead of focusing on simple causal relationships, aims to systematically construct more complex and coherent causal graphs ( Pearl 2000 ), incorporating all available evidence to form links between multiple variables. The CHIELD database, which is a repository of linguistic hypotheses produced in literature, was created to explore such graphs in order to find gaps or conflicting relationships, which can help to design empirical research addressing these issues and uncovering actual causal mechanisms ( Roberts et al. 2020 ). The database is public and functional: it is designed to be extendable by future researchers to ultimately become comprehensive and inclusive of as many languages as possible (that exist). Universal acceptance of a database such as this is the first step toward a realistic implementation of the maximum robustness approach.

To summarize, the current general trend for the linguistic field seems to move further and further away from notions of language innateness, although significant support for these viewpoints remains. In the scope of language evolution, usage-based frameworks allow for significantly more detailed and insightful investigations into the emergence of language. The same appears to be true for research into the modality of origin: this landscape of theories is also changing.

2.1. Modality of Origin

The debate on the modality of origin, which is the initial main channel carrying verbal information, also has a few contesting theories: according to the “gesture-first” view, “language evolved initially from manual gestures with vocal elements gradually added” ( Corballis 2011, p. 383 ). The “speech-first” view ( Dunbar 1997 ; MacNeilage 2008 ) argues for the pre-emergence of a vocal–auditory modality given its present-day dominance (for a full historical overview, see Fitch 2010 ). Modern theories argue for a multimodal emergence theory, incorporating complex interplay between auditory and visual channels ( Perlman 2017 ). Among newer ideas, “pantomime-first” was put forward as a distinct theoretical proposal ( Zlatev et al. 2017 ), which intrigues but does not provide much empirical evidence for its support. Another supporting usage-based account on multimodality comes from Levinson and Holler ( 2014 ), who propose that language normally occurs while embedded in a layered structure of multiple other channels of information. This view enables different phylogenetic and evolutionary origins to be assigned to each layer. Such holistic representation helps to bridge the gulf between the species, allowing us to recognize precursor adaptations such as turn-taking in current primates and the gestural skills of great apes as the first steps toward language formation, while the whole ensemble of language continues to be distinctively human.

One of the novel multimodal hypotheses is the mirror system hypothesis developed by Rizzolatti and Arbib ( 1998 ), which postulates that the mechanisms that support language in the human brain evolved atop a basic mechanism not originally related to communication: the mirror system, as the evolutionary basis for language, possesses a capacity to generate and recognize a set of actions. Arbib argues that the evolution of language is rooted in the execution and observation of hand movements, leading to the emergence of sign language, which was thereafter extended to speech. Complex imitation for hand movements evolved adaptively because of its utility in the social sharing of practical and manual skills. Skill sharing through imitation, such as grasping objects and using simple tools, existed long before language, being “more powerful than the call and gesture systems of nonhuman primates but lacking the full richness of modern human languages” ( Arbib 2012, p. 157 ).

Importantly, the origin theories based on the writing modality are characteristically absent, which is understandable given its (mostly) secondary nature to spoken language. This, however, is all too indicative of the attitudes to writing in language research prior to modern studies. The linguistic views on the emergence of writing were varied and controversial, echoing many general issues of the evolution of spoken language. The traditional outlook on writing systems since Aristotle was superficial, ostensibly viewing these systems as an optional, supplemental representation of spoken language. Moreover, writing was deemed a “wandering outcast of linguistics” ( Derrida 1976 ), leading to a suppression of research on writing. Similarly, for Saussure, written language was an object of suspicion, presenting a confounding and contaminating influence on language, going so far as to state that “to let go of the letter means a first step in the direction of truth” ( Saussure et al. 1986, p. 32 ). The views that were expressed during that time in the field were later ascribed to the “written language bias” ( Linell 2004 ).

Views that contested that bias started emerging in the mid-20th century from the historical ( Goody 1986 ) and anthropological ( McLuhan 1962 ) fields that, in turn, influenced studies of language to ascribe a more fundamental meaning to writing. One of the modern points of view from D.S. Olson proposed a special relationship of writing to the general machinery of language, which was influenced by those accounts and driven by developmental evidence ( Robinson et al. 1983 ). Olson’s most recent account postulated that reading and writing create a system of meta-representation concepts that contribute to consciousness, the formation of systematic thought, and rationality. In addition, some theories suggest that writing did not emerge as a secondary representation of spoken language but as the evolution of the token system for the purposes of goods exchanged or accounting ( Schmandt-Besserat 2012 ). The role of writing systems is similarly far from secondary according to the literacy hypothesis ( Goody and Watt 1963 ), and while it has received a lot of criticism on the matter of most aspects of civilized society preexisting and assimilating literacy at its advent, some scholars define influential “biases” ( Olson 2012 ) that may have contributed greatly to the cognitive and social development of the species. Such an impact of writing may be evidenced by tests of intelligence, including items that deal with vocabulary and the relationship between words, which test our capacity to participate in a literate environment ( Olson 2005 , as cited in Preiss and Sternberg 2006 ). Additionally, writing is essential to consider if adopting the adaptationist point of view, as the emergence of writing seems to possess several features characteristic of a Darwinian process ( Lock and Gers 2012 ).

Thus, while the dominating role of the vocal–auditory modality remains indisputable, progress in the field was made toward developing multimodal theories of language origin, which aid in unifying disparate evidence in support of different single-modality theories under a single governing principle.

2.2. Origin of Languages

The second question out of the pair laid out at the beginning of the section, namely, regarding “the origin of languages,” was mostly inquired upon in neighboring fields of inquiry and tied to the spread of human populations. A link between the human genome and the spread of languages has been debated ever since Darwin proposed that “a perfect pedigree of mankind… would afford the best classification of the various languages now spoken throughout the world” ( Darwin 1871 ). While some argued that the spread of languages is a good proxy for the dispersion of human populations ( Gray and Atkinson 2003 ; Mace and Holden 2005 ), opposition to this assumption was also persistent (e.g., Donohue and Denham 2010 ). Quantitative evidence supports both a general gene–language dispersion correspondence but also substantial (~20%) mismatches between 10 language families and corresponding populations ( Barbieri et al. 2022 ).

Globally, a consensus around the serial founder effect (SFE) process playing an important role in shaping global patterns of neutral genetic diversity is currently forming. This process entails a series of population splits, movements into an unoccupied territory, and subsequent isolation: beginning in Africa and proceeding through Eurasia into the Americas and Oceania. At the within-population level, it led to a steady decay in genetic diversity with increasing geographic distance from East Africa; at the between-population level, it led to a steady increase in genetic distance with increasing geographic distance ( Prugnolle et al. 2005 ; Ramachandran et al. 2005 ). The debate on the topic of language dispersion was later reignited by Atkinson ( 2011 ), who proposed that phoneme inventories in human languages had undergone a parallel SFE process based on the finding that the number of phonemes in 504 widespread languages decreased linearly with increasing geographic distance from Africa. Alternative assumptions for worldwide phonemic cline were tested using numerical simulations, showing that this pattern may be due to a repeated bottleneck effect and phonemic loss: low-density populations lost phonemes during the out-of-Africa dispersal of modern humans ( Pérez-Losada and Fort 2018 ). Creanza et al. ( 2015 ) further delved into this issue by performing joint and parallel analyses of phoneme counts in 2,082 languages and DNA microsatellite polymorphisms, which were used as signatures of human demographic history to calculate genetic distances between 246 populations. The results decisively vindicated Darwin’s proposal of human races and languages evolving in concert following a tree-like history of splits and isolation ( Darwin 1860 ) at the global level; however, it did not align with the SFE model with Africa as the center of origin, with it instead being more inclined toward a Eurasian-centered model. A more recent and novel analysis, which covered a cultural layer adjacent to language, namely, music, was carried out on a dataset of 152 societies (containing 1,054 songs from the public database The Global Jukebox in the form of raw coded Cantometrics data, 1,719 genomic profiles, and 152 languages); the analysis demonstrated weak links between music and language (R 2 <= 0.05), as well as with genetic distance and geographic proximity, in contrast to the much stronger relationships found between genes and geography: the results suggest that genes and culture are surprisingly decoupled ( Passmore et al. 2022 ). For the Indo-European family, Bouckaert et al. ( 2012 ) used Bayesian phylogeographic approaches with a dataset of basic vocabulary term lists from 103 ancient and contemporary Indo-European languages to model the expansion of the family, finding decisive support for an Anatolian origin over a steppe origin, with both the inferred timing and root location of the Indo-European language trees fitting with an agricultural expansion from Anatolia beginning 8000 to 9500 years ago. Certain linguistic methods, such as Bayesian phylogeographic approaches, that emerged from recent studies provided tentative answers to general questions of human prehistory: a recent study using lexical data and Bayesian phylogenetic methods placed the Austronesian origin in Taiwan approximately 5230 years ago and supported the hypothesis of “pulse-pause” expansion from Taiwan on the origin of the Austronesian settlers of the Pacific ( Gray et al. 2009 ). While being fairly recent for the field, the aforementioned techniques succeeded in helping linguists reclaim the issue of the origin of language as the viable research aim for future investigations aside from the origin of humanity research in neighboring fields.

To summarize, new technological and methodological advances have led to the most drastic changes in language evolution research. Large-scale investigations do require substantial resources, and interdisciplinary collaboration poses a challenge, but the results obtained contribute to significant advancements in the linguistic and neighboring fields in regard to the origin of language dispersal.

2.3. Neural Correlates of Language

The final important aspect of language origin studies, tangential to linguistics but central to psycholinguistics, is of utmost importance for the present essay: neural correlates of language. Evidence obtained from numerous previous studies that attempted to localize language within the brain is well established: the clinical studies of Broca ( Broca 1861 ) in the 19th century and Wernicke ( Wernicke [1874] 1994 ) in the 20th century, although contested now, served as an initial impulse for this. Similar to the debate on the language faculty origin in linguistic circles, we can note that the initial research findings that focused on narrow specificity of function were later extended to cover contesting evidence and new theoretical frameworks and have evolved into a multidimensional system. Through further publications on the topic emphasizing the importance of previously unaccounted-for brain regions (e.g., insula, Dronkers 1996 ), the model of choice for the end of the 20th century became the aphasia model (e.g., Obler and Gjerlow 1999 ). At the beginning of the 21st century, the model was further expanded into Broca-type and Wernicke-type aphasias in accordance with the impairment in one of two language comprehension axes ( Ardila 2011 , 2012 ), further solidifying the trend toward system complexity.

Modern models of language include numerous areas of the brain organized in multiple circuits within clusters of activation ( Ardila et al. 2016 ). One such model was constructed by Peter Hagoort ( 2005 ), who argued that the operation of distributed neural networks in Broca’s area and the left inferior frontal gyrus (LIFG) involves parallel processing of semantic, syntactic, and phonological information through three functional components: memory (long-term memory retrieval), unification (integrating information), and control (selecting a language “action”). Evidence from EEG and MEG studies helped to identify the specific temporal features of unification and memory retrieval components, arguing for neuronal synchronization that supports functional interrelatedness rather than strict domain specificity ( Bastiaansen and Hagoort 2006 ). These considerations are far from a theoretical conjecture nowadays, as they have been translated into presurgical planning ( Alemi et al. 2018 ). Additionally, there is evidence of neural multifunctionality for language networks, in particular, several frontal networks being linked to non-linguistic functions, such as mental rotation ( Jordan et al. 2001 ), musical syntax processing ( Maess et al. 2001 ), and arithmetic comprehension ( Baldo and Dronkers 2007 ). Such findings have driven researchers toward frameworks of multifunctional modularity and are instrumental for scholars developing usage-based approaches to language evolution.

Thus, despite initial findings that focused on the narrow specificity of function, the field has evolved to include numerous areas of the brain organized in multiple circuits within clusters of activation, in addition to the emerging evidence of neural multifunctionality for language networks.

3. Language Adaptation

Lupyan and Dale ( 2016 ) argued that observed linguistic differences arise not only from the accumulation of random changes due to the languages drifting apart but also may be reflective of the environment in which the language was developing. These environmental aspects that pressure languages into continuous diversification are social, physical, and technological in nature ( Lupyan and Dale 2016 ).

Just like birds develop different beaks adapting to different environments, languages and cultures might be undergoing similar changes ( Lupyan and Dale 2016 ). Charles Darwin, in “The Descent of Man, and Selection in Relation to Sex,” cited Max Müller to make a case for the evolution of language: “A struggle for life is constantly going on amongst the words and grammatical forms in each language. The better, the shorter, the easier forms are constantly gaining the upper hand, and they owe their success to their own inherent virtue.” ( Darwin 1871, p. 58 ). However, this idea of progress in linguistic evolution is considered dysfunctional by some ( Labov 1991 ; Mendívil-Giró 2018 ) due to its inability to explain the main patterns of linguistic structural diversity; a growing body of research asserts the contrary. The process of language diversification cannot be understood without considering the pressures that several factors (physical, ecological, and social) put on language users in different environments ( Bentz et al. 2018 ).

3.1. Ecological Adaptations

Similar to the communication systems of other species, language may be affected by ecological factors. Physiologically based predictions demonstrate that languages with complex tonality have generally not developed in very cold or otherwise desiccated climates, as air dryness decreases the control of the vocal folds and pitch production, and this, in turn, results in the absence of a (complex) tone system. The geographic–linguistic association operates within continents, major language families, and across language isolates ( Everett et al. 2015 ). However, replication of the study on a different dataset found it was not robust ( Roberts 2018 ). An analysis of over 4,000 language varieties showed a positive association between the language’s degree of reliance on vowels and the typical ambient humidity of a language’s native locale, which is consistent with other studies that focus on the link between aridity (i.e., the lack of effective moisture in a climate) and tonality of language ( Everett et al. 2015 ; Everett 2017 ), but the robustness was later found to be limited ( Roberts 2018 ).

Environments in which higher sound frequencies are less faithfully transmitted due to denser vegetation or higher ambient temperatures seem to be related to the greater use of sounds of lower frequencies (“more sonorous” languages). The results of Maddieson and Coupé ( 2015 ) point to a significant relationship between the “consonant-heaviness” of languages and several environmental factors, including tree cover and precipitation ( Maddieson and Coupé 2015 ). Further analysis of spoken samples did not find the relationship significant but identified that the percentage of sonorous material is correlated with the mean annual temperature in the area of the language ( Maddieson 2018 ). Studies that focused on the influence of temperature on languages find that languages spoken in cold, small regions tend to be more complex across a range of linguistic features, such as morphosyntactic complexity, linguistic diversity, word length, and consonant inventory ( Lewis and Frank 2016 ).

Another striking example is the observed partial correlation between latitude and the absence or presence of the word for the color blue ( Brown and Lindsey 2004 ; Lindsey and Brown 2002 ) due to the negative impact of ultraviolet light (UV-B) on the perception of the blue/green distinction (phototoxicity). In high-UV areas, languages without the word for blue prevail, which also correlates with the rates of blue-yellow color vision deficiency in these areas suggesting an evolutionary, physiological cause for both phenomena ( Brown and Lindsey 2004 ; Dediu et al. 2017 ).

A common criticism of the abovementioned studies is that they are correlational in nature, thus, do not contribute to the understanding of possible mechanisms that underlie linguistic evolutionary processes. In order to improve the methodological robustness of the studies, additional approaches, such as iterated learning, a historical case study, corpus studies, and studying individual speech, was suggested ( Roberts 2018 ). For this reason, several studies tried to experimentally investigate how environmental factors drive the emergence of linguistic conventions. Nölle et al. ( 2020a ) adapted the classical maze game task to confirm that subtle environmental motivations cause the emergence of different communicative conventions in an otherwise identical task, pointing to linguistic adaptations being highly sensitive to factors of the shared task environment. The authors speculated that these kinds of mechanisms identified at a local interactional level might contribute to the systematic global variation observed between different languages.

One of the most striking examples of linguistic adaptation to the environment is whistled languages. The main purpose of whistled languages is to facilitate spoken communication at great distances, but it is also used in other circumstances, such as secrecy, courtship, singing, and communication in noisy environments. Although they are always referred to as languages, they are considered a mode of speech because whistled languages are always based on a spoken language ( Meyer 2015 ).

Several hypotheses were put forward to explain the current existence of whistled languages. One of them posits that whistled languages are simply the vestigial remains of a widespread ancient phenomenon. This mode of speech could have been used by prehistoric hunter-gatherers for hunting in groups or signaling a danger in any type of environment ( Nettle and Romaine 2000 ). Another possible explanation is that the actual whistled languages are found only in a small minority of languages due to the erosion of traditional lifestyles and the relative ease of resorting to shouting because whistled speech would generally require more pressure to develop. This argument would be in favor of a key role played by significant environmental constraints in the emergence of whistled speech, which is supported by the observed systematic adaptation of whistled speech to typically constraining and geographically scattered ecological milieux ( Meyer 2015 ).

It is estimated that approximately 70–80 languages actively use their whistled mode of speech, but the number is rapidly declining due to modern technologies of communication, and most of them are endangered ( Meyer 2018 ). Evidently, whistled speech plays a strong functional role by complementing regular speech under unusual circumstances. Around the world, whistled forms of languages are associated with traditional activities, such as hunting, hill agriculture, or shepherding, in which individuals are relatively isolated and scattered across substantial areas of densely vegetated landscapes. In this type of environment, whistling has a clear advantage over speaking or shouting: acoustic signals can easily overcome ambient conditions and can travel longer distances. For example, La Gomera, one of Spain’s Canary Islands, holds the record for the longest distance of whistled conversations of approximately 1 km ( Meyer 2015 ); others have observed communications at approximately 8 km ( Busnel and Classe 1976 ).

The principle of whistled speech is straightforward: people articulate words while whistling, which involves acoustic reduction at the produced frequency level and selection of key salient phonetic cues for the corresponding spoken utterances. The resulting signal’s linguistic structure is identical to standard speech. Interestingly, even though the acoustic channel is reduced, whistled sentences remain highly intelligible to trained speakers ( Meyer 2015 ).

It was suggested that human whistled languages can serve as a model for understanding the coding of information in dolphin whistle communication. Comparing human and dolphin whistles could become a complementary test bench for the development of new methodologies for decoding whistled communication signals by providing new perspectives on structural and organizational aspects of encoding information ( Meyer et al. 2021 ).

Overall, exploring the connection between the structure of languages and the environment in which they are utilized is complicated by several issues. If the ecology of the area was able to influence the language at the stage of its emergence, the amount of information needed to make a conclusive statement about it is scarce. Additionally, most of the studies that focus on the link between environment and language are correlational in nature. Although the overall structural diversity of languages has not been linked with other types of diversity, some aspects, such as morphosyntactic complexity or consonant/vowel inventory, may be affected.

3.2. Socio-Demographic Adaptations

Apart from the effects of the physical environment and location, languages may be shaped by social and demographic factors. A statistical analysis of 2000 languages revealed strong relationships between the morphological complexity of a language and demographic/socio-historical factors, including the number of language users, geographic spread, and degree of language contact ( Lupyan and Dale 2010 ). It was suggested that languages spoken by large groups have a simpler inflectional morphology than languages spoken by smaller groups. Additionally, languages spoken by large groups are more likely to utilize lexical strategies in place of inflectional morphology when encoding evidentiality, negation, aspect, and possession ( Lupyan and Dale 2010 ). Based on these findings, Dale and Lupyan proposed the linguistic niche hypothesis , which describes the esoteric and exoteric niches for languages. The exoteric linguistic niche includes languages with large numbers of speakers (e.g., English, Swahili, and Hindi), which forces these languages to serve as a means of communication between strangers. Speakers of languages in the exoteric niche, compared with the esoteric niche, are more likely to be non-native speakers or have learned the language from non-native speakers and use the language to speak to individuals from different ethnic and/or linguistic backgrounds. The esoteric niche includes languages like Tatar, Elfdalian, and Algonquin ( Dale and Lupyan 2012 ; Lupyan and Dale 2010 , 2016 ). Linguistically, esoteric languages are more likely to be classified as isolating rather than fusional, have fewer grammatical categories marked on the verb, are more likely to encode negation via analytical strategies than using inflections, are less likely to have indefinite and definite articles, and are less likely to communicate distance distinction demonstratives ( Lupyan and Dale 2010 ). Further studies found only limited support for this hypothesis ( Lewis and Frank 2016 ) or did not find a strong relationship between the grammatical or statistical structure of language and the proportion of non-native speakers ( Koplenig 2019 ).

Winters et al. ( 2015 ) experimentally investigated the role of the communicative situation in which an utterance is produced and how it influences the emergence of three types of linguistic systems: underspecified languages, holistic systems, and systematic languages. Using a discrimination task in a communication game and manipulating whether the feature dimension shape was relevant or not in discriminating between two referents, it was established that different linguistic systems emerged. Furthermore, experimental languages gradually developed to encode information relevant to the communicative task in a given situational context. These results suggest that language systems adapt to their contextual niche over iterated learning.

Another interesting observation was made about the influence of population size on rates of language evolution. The rates of gain and loss of cognate words for basic vocabulary were analyzed in Polynesian languages. Larger populations were observed to have higher rates of gain of new words, while smaller populations had higher rates of word loss, which suggests that demographic factors may affect rates of language evolution and that rates of gain and loss are affected in different ways. However, the authors found that the results were strikingly consistent with general predictions of evolutionary models, paralleling positive selection in the case of greater rates of word gain in larger populations, and loss of diversity in small populations and greater rates of word loss ( Bromham et al. 2015 ).

An inquiry into language evolution that was made using estimates of cognate replacement for 200 concepts on an Indo-European language tree spanning 6–10 millennia to measure lexical evolution rates demonstrated that negative valence correlates with faster cognate replacement, even while controlling for frequency of use. Follow-up analyses showed that it is most robust for adjectives, does not consistently reach statistical significance for verbs, and never reaches significance for nouns ( Jackson et al. 2023 ).

Socio-demographic pressures are also known to lead to the emergence of new languages, the investigation of which can shed light on the process of language evolution. Among the newest languages of the world, Afrikaans is the youngest nationally recognized language, whose origin is tied to the establishment of Cape Colony in the Cape of Good Hope on the land of indigenous Khoekhoen people in the mid-16th century. The starting point of the Cape Colony is the landing of Jan van Riebeeck with three vessels to the Cape on 6th April 1652, which can also be thought of as the starting point of the Afrikaans language ( De Villiers 2012 ). The language developed through to the 18th century, becoming the language with the widest geographical, demographic, and racial distribution of all official languages of South Africa ( Webb 2003 ), and the debate on its origin was live and ongoing until the 20th century, mainly due the clash of political and ideological views that it instigated. Certain scholars at the time denied any indigenous influences on the language, while others insisted that the language was as much creolized as it was a product of West-Germanic sources. It has its roots in 17th-century Dutch but has been influenced by English, French, and German ( Hamans 2021 ), with traces of, amongst others, Malay and Portuguese ( Conradie and Groenewald 2014 ), and influenced by the pidgin talk of the indigenous Khoi and the San ( Hamans 2021 ).

While not as widely recognized as Afrikaans, Light Warlpiri would constitute the newest example of an emerging language. This language was discovered and documented by Carmel O’Shannessy ( 2005 ) and is thought to have originated sometime at the end of the previous century. It was spoken in the Warlpiri community of Lajamanu, in the Northern Territory of Australia, by children and young adults who are now mostly approximately 40 years of age ( O’Shannessy and Brown 2021 ). The language systematically combines elements of Warlpiri (a Pama-Nyungan language), Kriol (an English-based creole), and English, and was derived from the code-switched speech of parents to children following a particular pattern, where a Kriol pronoun and verb were inserted into a Warlpiri string, as part of a baby talk register ( O’Shannessy 2012 ). A new language emerged when young children internalized this pattern of speech as a single system, distinct from Warlpiri ( O’Shannessy 2020 ). In its formation, a speech pattern was further innovated in the verbal auxiliary system, namely, the =m “NONFUTURE” suffix ( O’Shannessy 2013 ). Light Warlpiri has enough systematic evidence to distinguish it as a separate language, for which its precursors act as lexifiers ( O’Shannessy 2005 ), and is nowadays the language of everyday interaction of the adult generation in the Lajamanu community.

To conclude, most typological studies suggest that linguistic diversity may be affected by several demographical and sociolinguistic factors. Social contexts can influence the way language is acquired and used, leading to linguistic structures that are specific to certain groups. Over time, this can result in variations in language usage that are reflected in typological patterns. This shows that language is not just a set of fixed rules and structures but rather a dynamic and adaptive skill that is shaped by the social context in which it is used. This ability to adapt to a social environment is a key feature of a language, and it helps to explain how language has evolved to meet the changing needs and contexts of different communities.

3.3. Technological Adaptations

Technology has drastically affected language and given rise to what is now commonly referred to as “text-speak” ( Al-Sharqi and Abbasi 2020 ). One of the biggest changes technology brought about is the speed of communication. The human brain is forced to process an unending stream of linguistic input and respond to it immediately. Christiansen and Charter called this the “Now-or-Never bottleneck,” which describes the immediacy with which the brain must compress and recode linguistic input ( Christiansen and Chater 2016 ). This bottleneck acts as a strong selection pressure against words and grammatical construction parsing, which, in real-time, is nearly impossible, especially when pressure is being put on the written language to communicate subtle nuances of face-to-face communication. The multi-faceted pressure inevitably influences the language that now has to undergo significant adaptive processes in order to fit the requirements of modern times, turning into what some deem “a natural experiment in the development of written communication” ( Varnhagen et al. 2010 ). Some of the ways that this adaptation manifests in the language are novel conventions of online communication, including acronyms, the modified use of typographic marks, and the use of emojis ( Lupyan and Dale 2016 ).

Emojis and emoticons are a group of symbolic combinations or pictures that are characteristic of online communication. An emoji (“☹”) is a graphic symbol that represents a wide variety of different things, ranging from complex facial expressions to concepts and ideas. It is thought to have developed from emoticons, i.e., representations of facial expressions usually comprised of various combinations of keyboard characters (“:)”). These symbols usually augment a message with non-verbal elements ( Novak et al. 2015 ).

Due to their growing popularity, emojis are used not only in online communications but are becoming integrated into an increasingly wider variety of contexts. Specifically, research is conducted to understand how emoji-enriched interfaces affect performance in the classroom ( Aliannejadi et al. 2021 ), marketing and advertisement ( Lee et al. 2021 ), and even their implications in law ( Goldman 2018 ).

Emojis (or “smileys”) are a unique phenomenon in terms of their nature and diverse functions in communication. On the one hand, emojis produce effects that are functionally similar to the response observed for facial expressions of emotion in face-to-face communication. They seem to affect the perceived emotional intensity of a message and accentuate its perceived valence by acting as nonverbal cues in digital communication ( Erle et al. 2021 ). On the other hand, emojis are closely connected with words. It was shown that the time course of semantic congruency effects on eye movements for emojis is similar to effects that were previously shown for words ( Barach et al. 2021 ). In the online public context, emojis alter the lexical diversity of text, which may point to a compensatory relationship between emojis and words in communication ( Feldman et al. 2021 ). Additionally, there is a link between emojis and gestures, with emojis denoting objects and activities interacting with logical operators in a text in a similar way as gestures do with speech ( Pierini 2021 ).

Lupyan and Dale argued that the divergence between conventional written languages (as well as online written communication) differs in many ways from the divergence between conventional spoken languages, for example, Dutch and Afrikaans. Both of these phenomena represent how languages (or language registers) adapt to the environments in which they are being used ( Lupyan and Dale 2016 ). Similar to Afrikaans and Dutch, it is feasible to assume that the written form of language diverges from the spoken form as an adaptation to this new environment, and the online form combines the features of the two forms into something that linguist John McWhorter described as a pure “linguistic miracle happening right under our noses” ( McWhorter 2013 ).

Shortcuts are one of the most prevalent features of the new “netspeak” ( Varnhagen et al. 2010 ). Common shortcuts in the context of instant messaging and online communication include abbreviations (prof—professor), initialisms and acronyms (LOL—laughing out loud, ASAP—as soon as possible), and logograms or “alphanumeronyms” (CUL8R—see you later).

One of the explanations of this process posits that users intuitively ignore uneconomical language rules and strive for cost-effectiveness, increasing the efficiency of the language orthography ( Lančarič 2016 ) and enriching it with new words and phrases that express complex feelings, emotions, or reactions (for example “wowzy” usually stands for extreme amazement or awe of a situation, thing, person, or place). Interestingly, some of the recently introduced units started to undergo a process of pragmaticalization, a subclass of grammaticalization, which possesses many similar features of grammaticalization processes but is distinguished from other subtypes by specific functions, domains, and syntactic integration ( Diewald 2011 ).

Pragmaticalized units may partially or completely lose their semantic meaning and move into a new pragmatic domain of function and meaning. In this sense, online communication is not only enriched by the spoken form of language with its abundance of discourse (or pragmatic) markers but gives rise to new netspeak-specific pragmatic features that slowly pave their way back into spoken language, making this interaction bidirectional and mutually enriching. This is only one of many aspects that is indicative of the emergence of a unique new hybrid register that fuses the full range of variants from the language use, namely, written, spoken, formal, informal, and vernacular variants ( Tagliamonte and Denis 2008 ).

One particular field able to inform the shortcut trend is quantitative linguistics, one of the aims of which is the development of statistical laws about language usage. Such laws can tell us a lot about speech and language efficiency principles, the most established among them being Zipf’s law of word frequency, which quantifies the frequency of occurrence of words, demonstrating that there is no unarbitrary way to distinguish between rare and common words ( Zipf 1949 ). This law is also rather common in complex systems where discrete units self-organize into groups or types ( Corral et al. 2019 ). Zipf’s law of brevity is sufficiently easier to observe through personal experience, stating that more frequent words tend to be shorter, and rarer words tend to be longer ( Bentz and Ferrer-i-Cancho 2016 ). A functional explanation for this law suggested by Zipf is the law of least effort, stating that it is human nature to want the greatest outcome at the least amount of work. Closely related to it is the Menzerath–Altmann law, which postulates that the size of the constituents (e.g., phonemes) of a construction (e.g., morpheme) decreases with the increasing size of the construction ( Altmann 1980 ). These two laws suggest that in human vocal communication, the maximization of coding efficiency and minimization of code length act as selective pressures to compress the elements supporting information ( Favaro et al. 2020 ). In addition to spoken language, there is evidence that laws of brevity hold in most writing systems (in a sample of 1262 texts and 986 different languages, see Bentz and Ferrer-i-Cancho 2016 ). However, there are exceptions, as with figurative signals, the frequency of which is shown to be positively correlated with complexity ( Miton and Morin 2019 ). The suggestion that these laws might extend beyond human language is substantivized by recent studies into vocal sequences of non-human primates ( Semple et al. 2010 ) and penguins ( Favaro et al. 2020 ), while some studies challenge that notion ( Bezerra et al. 2011 ).

Hence, technology has had a significant impact on language in recent years, giving rise to new forms of communication and new ways of using language. One of the most notable changes brought about by technology is the emergence of “text-speak,” which is a form of language that is characterized by the use of shorthand, abbreviations, and symbols in text-based communication. In this way, digital communication can be seen as a distinct form of language with unique features and conventions that are specific to the digital environment. It is not a replacement for spoken or written language, but rather, it is an additional way of communicating and conveying meaning, which is a skill that emerged in humans as a response to a new and all-encompassing technological environment with its constraints and possibilities.

4. Conclusions

The questions of the origin and evolution of language, apart from gaining new evidence for their resolution through the use of novel linguistic and psycholinguistic methods and interdisciplinary inquiries, also changed in their nature. Modern language research strays away from portraying human language as a unique anthropoid phenomenon, the expression of which has not been modified by selection pressure, instead viewing it through the lens of ongoing human evolution and strict adherence to communicative goals. In this light, language can be viewed as a toolbox, the contents of which change in accordance with the needs of the human species, which is the statement that we attempted to demonstrate through the review of the literature on language evolution and adaptation.

Manifold examples of languages adapting to and reflecting different aspects of the environment illustrate that linguistic diversification is not simply an accumulation of random changes over time. The most recent example of strong selective pressure affecting languages all over the world is the integration of instant communication technologies. This new phenomenon allows us to witness language modification in real time and better understand the underlying processes. It can already be confidently stated that human language is undergoing one of its most massive changes at this very moment while following the essential principles that governed its existence and development before the onset of the digital age.

Multimodal and usage-based emergence theories, in addition to more robust correlation and causation links, provide a framework that is apt to incorporate the majority of scientific knowledge about language. Modern neural correlate models of language processing further serve to illustrate the interconnectedness of language to other domains of cognition. Further steps in that direction of inquiry may only serve to elucidate how thoroughly integrated language is with all other types of human behavior.

We propose that the study of languages should not be confined to properties of particular languages and language in general but should incorporate a wider array of contributing factors that inevitably shape the way different species, including humans, communicate. This all-encompassing approach will provide more insights into the nature, structure, and functions of language in diverse environments and demographic contexts, as well as help to explain the way human communication adapts to and transforms in response to the pressures put forward by technological breakthroughs and societal transformations, along with the alterations in our species’ ecological niche in the Anthropocene era.

Funding Statement

The preparation of this essay was supported, in part, by a grant (R01HD109307) from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD), USA, to the University of Houston (principal investigator: Elena L. Grigorenko) and by funds from Sirius University, Russia. The content is solely our responsibility and does not necessarily reflect the views of the funders.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, I.M., K.K. and E.L.G.; investigation, I.M. and K.K.; resources, E.L.G.; writing—original draft preparation, I.M. and K.K.; writing—review and editing, I.M., K.K. and E.L.G.; supervision, E.L.G.; funding acquisition, E.L.G. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Informed consent statement, data availability statement, conflicts of interest.

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

  • Alemi Razieh, Batouli Seyed Amir Hossein, Behzad Ebrahim, Ebrahimpoor Mitra, Oghabian Mohammad Ali. Not single brain areas but a network is involved in language: Applications in presurgical planning. Clinical Neurology and Neurosurgery. 2018; 165 :116–28. doi: 10.1016/j.clineuro.2018.01.009. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Aliannejadi Mohammad, Landoni Monica, Huibers Theo, Murgia Emiliana, Pera Maria Soledad. Children’s Perspective on How Emojis Help Them to Recognise Relevant Results: Do Actions Speak Louder Than Words?; Paper presented at 2021 Conference on Human Information Interaction and Retrieval; Canberra, Australia. March 19; 2021. pp. 301–5. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Al-Sharqi Laila, Abbasi Irum Saeed. The Influence of Technology on English Language and Literature. English Language Teaching. 2020; 13 :7. doi: 10.5539/elt.v13n7p1. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Altmann Gabriel. Prolegomena to Menzerath’s law. Glottometrika. 1980; 2 :1–10. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Anthropocene Working Group What Is the Anthropocene?—Current Definition and Status. 2019. [(accessed on 16 March 2023)]. Available online: http://quaternary.stratigraphy.org/working-groups/anthropocene/
  • Arbib Michael A. How the Brain Got Language: The Mirror System Hypothesis. Oxford University Press; Oxford: 2012. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ardila Alfredo, Bernal Byron, Rosselli Monica. How Localized are Language Brain Areas? A Review of Brodmann Areas Involvement in Oral Language. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 2016; 31 :112–22. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acv081. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ardila Alfredo. There are two different language systems in the brain. Journal of Behavioral and Brain Science. 2011; 1 :23. doi: 10.4236/jbbs.2011.12005. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ardila Alfredo. Interaction between lexical and grammatical language systems in the brain. Physics of Life Reviews. 2012; 9 :198–214. doi: 10.1016/j.plrev.2012.05.001. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Atkinson Quentin D. Phonemic Diversity Supports a Serial Founder Effect Model of Language Expansion from Africa. Science. 2011; 332 :346–49. doi: 10.1126/science.1199295. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Baldo Juliana V., Dronkers Nina F. Neural correlates of arithmetic and language comprehension: A common substrate? Neuropsychologia. 2007; 45 :229–35. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.07.014. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Barach Elisa, Feldman Laurie Beth, Sheridan Heather. Are emojis processed like words? Eye movements reveal the time course of semantic processing for emojified text. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2021; 28 :978–91. doi: 10.3758/s13423-020-01864-y. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Barbieri Chiara, Blasi Damián E., Arango-Isaza Epifanía, Sotiropoulos Alexandros G., Hammarström Harald, Wichmann Søren, Greenhill Simon J., Gray Russell D., Forkel Robert, Bickel Balthasar, et al. A global analysis of matches and mismatches between human genetic and linguistic histories. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2022; 119 :e2122084119. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2122084119. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bastiaansen Marcel, Hagoort Peter. Oscillatory neuronal dynamics during language comprehension. In: Neuper Christa, Klimesch Wolfgang., editors. Progress in Brain Research. vol. 159. Elsevier; Amsterdam: 2006. pp. 179–96. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bentz Christian, Ferrer-i-Cancho Ramon. Zipf’s Law of Abbreviation as a Language Universal. University of Tübingen; Tübingen: 2016. pp. 1–4. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bentz Christian, Dediu Dan, Verkerk Annemarie, Jäger Gerhard. The evolution of language families is shaped by the environment beyond neutral drift. Nature Human Behaviour. 2018; 2 :11. doi: 10.1038/s41562-018-0457-6. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bezerra Bruna M., Souto Antonio S., Radford Andrew N., Jones Gareth. Brevity Is Not Always a Virtue in Primate Communication. Biology Letters. 2011; 7 :23–25. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2010.0455. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Boeckx Cedric. Language Science Press. Language Science Press; Berlin: 2021. Reflections on language evolution. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bolhuis Johan J., Tattersall Ian, Chomsky Noam, Berwick Robert C. How Could Language Have Evolved? PLoS Biology. 2014; 12 :e1001934. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1001934. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bouckaert Remco R., Lemey Philippe, Dunn Michael D., Greenhill Simon J., Alekseyenko Alexander V., Drummond Alexei J., Gray Russell D., Suchard Marc A., Atkinson Quentin D. Mapping the origins and expansion of the Indo-European language family. Science. 2012; 337 :957–60. doi: 10.1126/science.1219669. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Broca Paul. Remarks on the seat of the faculty of articulated language, following an observation of aphemia (loss of speech) Bulletin de La Société Anatomique. 1861; 6 :330–57. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bromham Lindell, Hua Xia, Fitzpatrick Thomas G., Greenhill Simon J. Rate of language evolution is affected by population size. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2015; 112 :2097–102. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1419704112. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Brown Angela M., Lindsey Delvin T. Color and language: Worldwide distribution of Daltonism and distinct words for “blue” Visual Neuroscience. 2004; 21 :409–12. doi: 10.1017/S0952523804213098. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Busnel Rene-Guy, Classe André. Whistled Languages. Springer; Berlin and Heidelberg: 1976. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Chomsky Noam. Language and Problems of Knowledge: The Managua Lectures. MIT Press; Cambridge: 1988. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Christiansen Morten H., Chater Nick. The Now-or-Never bottleneck: A fundamental constraint on language. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 2016; 39 :e62. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X1500031X. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Conradie Jac, Groenewald Gerald. In: Die ontstaan en vestiging van Afrikaans. Carstens W. A. M., Bosman N., editors. Van Schaik; Pretoria: 2014. pp. 27–60. Kontemporêre Afrikaanse Taalkunde. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Corballis Michael C. The Origins of Language in Manual Gestures. Oxford University Press; Oxford: 2011. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Corral Álvaro, Serra Isabel, Ferrer-i-Cancho Ramon. Distinct flavors of Zipf’s law and its maximum likelihood fitting: Rank-size and size-distribution representations. ArXiv: Data Analysis, Statistics and Probability. 2019; 102 :052113. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevE.102.052113. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Creanza Nicole, Ruhlen Merritt, Pemberton Trevor J., Rosenberg Noah A., Feldman Marcus W., Ramachandran Sohini. A comparison of worldwide phonemic and genetic variation in human populations. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2015; 112 :1265–72. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1424033112. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dale Rick, Lupyan Gary. Understanding the origins of morphological diversity: The linguistic niche hypothesis. Advances in Complex Systems. 2012; 15 :1150017. doi: 10.1142/S0219525911500172. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Darwin Charles. On the Origin of Species by Means of Natural Selection: Or the Preservation of the Favoured Races in the Struggle for Life. John Murray; London: 1860. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Darwin Charles. The Descent of Man, and Selection in Relation to Sex, Vol. 1. John Murray; London: 1871. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • De Villiers Johan. Die Nederlandse era aan die Kaap, 1652–1806. In: Pretorius Fransjohan., editor. Geskiedenis van Suid-Afrika. Van Voortye Tot Vandag Bl. Tafelberg Publishers Ltd.; Cape Town: 2012. pp. 39–62. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dediu Dan, Boer Bart de. Language evolution needs its own journal. Journal of Language Evolution. 2016; 1 :1–6. doi: 10.1093/jole/lzv001. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dediu Dan, Janssen Rick, Moisik Scott R. Language is not isolated from its wider environment: Vocal tract influences on the evolution of speech and language. Language & Communication. 2017; 54 :9–20. doi: 10.1016/j.langcom.2016.10.002. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Derrida Jacques. Of Grammatology, Trans. Gayatri Chakravorty Spivak. Johns Hopkins University Press; Baltimore: 1976. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Diewald Gabriele. Pragmaticalization (Defined) as Grammaticalization of Discourse Functions. vol. 49. De Gruyter Mouton; Berlin: 2011. pp. 365–90. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Donohue Mark, Denham Tim. Farming and Language in Island Southeast Asia Reframing Austronesian History. Current Anthropology. 2010; 51 :223–56. doi: 10.1086/650991. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dronkers Nina F. A new brain region for coordinating speech articulation. Nature. 1996; 384 :159–61. doi: 10.1038/384159a0. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dunbar Robin I. M. Groups, Gossip, and the Evolution of Language. In: Schmitt Alain, Atzwanger Klaus, Grammer Karl, Schäfer Katrin., editors. New Aspects of Human Ethology. Springer; New York: 1997. pp. 77–89. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Erle Thorsten M., Schmid Karoline, Goslar Simon H., Martin Jared D. Emojis as social information in digital communication. Emotion. 2021; 22 :1529–1543. doi: 10.1037/emo0000992. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Everett Caleb, Blasi Damián E., Roberts Seán G. Climate, vocal folds, and tonal languages: Connecting the physiological and geographic dots. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2015; 112 :1322–27. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1417413112. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Everett Caleb. Languages in Drier Climates Use Fewer Vowels. Frontiers in Psychology. 2017; 8 :1285. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01285. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Favaro Livio, Gamba Marco, Cresta Eleonora, Fumagalli Elena, Bandoli Francesca, Pilenga Cristina, Isaja Valentina, Mathevon Nicolas, Reby David. Do penguins’ vocal sequences conform to linguistic laws? Biology Letters. 2020; 16 :20190589. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2019.0589. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Feldman Laurie, Barach Eliza, Srinivasan Vidhushini, Shaikh Samira. Emojis and Words Work Together in the Service of Communication. Association for the Advancement of Artificial Intelligence; Palo Alto: 2021. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Fitch William T. The Evolution of Language. Cambridge University Press; Cambridge: 2010. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Formigari Lia. L’origine del linguaggio. Ricognizioni storiche e valenze epistemologiche. In: Emanuele B., editor. Sull’origine del Linguaggio e delle lingue storico-naturali. Un confronto tra linguisti e non linguisti. Bulzoni; Roma: 2013. pp. 13–22. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Goldman Eric. Emojis and the Law. Washington Law Review. 2018; 93 :1227. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Goody Jack, Watt Ian. The Consequences of Literacy. Comparative Studies in Society and History. 1963; 5 :304–45. doi: 10.1017/S0010417500001730. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Goody Jack. The Logic of Writing and the Organization of Society , 1st ed. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 1986 doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511621598. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Graffi Giorgio. Origin of language and origin of languages. Evolutionary Linguistic Theory. 2019; 1 :6–23. doi: 10.1075/elt.00002.gra. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gray Russell D., Drummond Alexei J., Greenhill Simon J. Language Phylogenies Reveal Expansion Pulses and Pauses in Pacific Settlement. Science. 2009; 323 :479–83. doi: 10.1126/science.1166858. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gray Russell D., Atkinson Quentin D. Language-tree divergence times support the Anatolian theory of Indo-European origin. Nature. 2003; 426 :435–39. doi: 10.1038/nature02029. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Grigorenko Elena. The Never-Ending Innovativeness of the Wise Man. In: Preiss David D., Kaufman James C., Singer Marcos., editors. Innovation, Creativity and Change Across Cultures. Palgrave-Macmillan; London: 2023. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hagoort Peter. On Broca, brain, and binding: A new framework. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2005; 9 :416–23. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.07.004. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hamans Camiel. Afrikaans: A language where ideology and linguistics meet. Scripta Neophilologica Posnaniensia. 2021; 21 :15–92. doi: 10.14746/snp.2021.21.02. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hauser Mark D., Chomsky Noam, Fitch William T. The faculty of language: What is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? Science. 2002; 298 :1569–79. doi: 10.1126/science.298.5598.1569. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Jackson Joshua Conrad, Lindquist Kristen, Drabble Ryan, Atkinson Quentin, Watts Joseph. Valence-dependent mutation in lexical evolution. Nature Human Behaviour. 2023; 7 :190–99. doi: 10.1038/s41562-022-01483-8. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Winters James, Kirby Simon, Smith Kenny. Languages adapt to their contextual niche. Language and Cognition. 2015; 7 :415–49. doi: 10.1017/langcog.2014.35. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Jordan Kirsten, Heinze H. -J., Lutz Kai, Kanowski Martin, Jäncke Lutz. Cortical activations during the mental rotation of different visual objects. Neuroimage. 2001; 13 :143–52. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0677. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Koplenig Alexander. Language structure is influenced by the number of speakers but seemingly not by the proportion of non-native speakers. Royal Society Open Science. 2019; 6 :181274. doi: 10.1098/rsos.181274. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Labov William. Sociolinguistic Patterns. University of Philadelphia Press; Philadelphia: 1991. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lančarič Daniel. Sentential Acronyms in Informal Online Communication. Reviewed Conference Proceedings from an International Scientific Conference. 2016. [(accessed on 2 December 2022)]. pp. 12–17. Available online: https://faj.euba.sk/www_write/files/veda-vyskum/konferencie/zborniky/Cudzie_jazyky_v_premenach_casu_7_2016.pdf#page=10
  • Lee Jungwoo, Kim Cheong, Lee Kun Chang. Investigating the Negative Effects of Emojis in Facebook Sponsored Ads for Establishing Sustainable Marketing in Social Media. Sustainability. 2021; 13 :4864. doi: 10.3390/su13094864. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Levinson Stephen C., Holler Judith. The origin of human multi-modal communication. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B. 2014; 369 :20130302. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2013.0302. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lewis Molly, Frank Michael C. Linguistic niches emerge from pressures at multiple timescales. Cognitive Science. 2016. [(accessed on 25 November 2022)]. pp. 1385–90. Available online: https://langcog.stanford.edu/papers_new/lewis-2016-cogsci.pdf
  • Lewis Paul M. Ethnologue: Languages of the World. Linguistic Society of America; New York: 2009. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lindsey Delvin T., Brown Angela M. Color Naming and the Phototoxic Effects of Sunlight on the Eye. Psychological Science. 2002; 13 :506–12. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00489. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Linell Per. The Written Language Bias in Linguistics: Its Nature, Origins and Transformations. Routledge; London: 2004. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lock Andy, Gers Matt. Writing: A Mosaic of New Perspectives. Psychology Press; New York: 2012. The cultural evolution of written language and its effects: A Darwinian process from prehistory to the modern day; pp. 11–35. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lupyan Gary, Dale Rick. Language Structure Is Partly Determined by Social Structure. PLoS ONE. 2010; 5 :e8559. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0008559. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lupyan Gary, Dale Rick. Why Are There Different Languages? The Role of Adaptation in Linguistic Diversity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2016; 20 :649–60. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2016.07.005. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Mace Ruth, Holden Clare. A phylogenetic approach to cultural evolution. Trends in Ecology and Evolution. 2005; 20 :116–21. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2004.12.002. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • MacNeilage Peter F. The Origin of Speech. Oxford University Press; Oxford: 2008. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Maddieson I. Language Adapts to Environment: Sonority and Temperature. Frontiers in Communication. 2018; 3 :28. doi: 10.3389/fcomm.2018.00028. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Maddieson Ian, Coupé Christophe. Human spoken language diversity and the acoustic adaptation hypothesis. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 2015; 138 :1838–38. doi: 10.1121/1.4933848. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Maess Burkhard, Koelsch Stefan, Gunter Thomas C, Friederici Angela D. Musical syntax is processed in Broca’s area: An MEG study. Nature Neuroscience. 2001; 4 :540–45. doi: 10.1038/87502. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • McLuhan Marshall. The Gutenberg Galaxy: The Making of Typographic Man. University of Toronto Press; Toronto: 1962. [ Google Scholar ]
  • McWhorter John. John McWhorter: Txtng is killing language. JK!!!|TED Talk. 2013. [(accessed on 28 November 2022)]. Available online: https://www.ted.com/talks/john_mcwhorter_txtng_is_killing_language_jk
  • Mendívil-Giró José-Luis. Why Don’t Languages Adapt to Their Environment? Frontiers in Communication. 2018; 3 :24. doi: 10.3389/fcomm.2018.00024. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Meyer Julien, Magnasco Marcelo O., Reiss Diana. The Relevance of Human Whistled Languages for the Analysis and Decoding of Dolphin Communication. Frontiers in Psychology. 2021; 12 :689501. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.689501. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Meyer Julien. Whistled Languages. Springer; Berlin: 2015. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Meyer Julien. The Routledge Handbook of Language Revitalization. Routledge; London: 2018. Revitalization of Whistled Languages; p. 552. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Miton Helen, Morin Olivier. When iconicity stands in the way of abbreviation: No Zipfian effect for figurative signals. PLoS ONE. 2019; 14 :e0220793. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0220793. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Nettle Daniel, Romaine Suzanne. Vanishing Voices: The Extinction of the World’s Languages. Oxford University Press; Oxford: 2000. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Nölle Jonas, Fusaroli Riccardo, Mills Gregory J., Tylén Kristian. Language as shaped by the environment: Linguistic construal in a collaborative spatial task. Palgrave Communications. 2020a; 6 :27. doi: 10.1057/s41599-020-0404-9. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Nölle Jonas, Hartmann Stefan, Tinits Peeter. Language Evolution Research in the Year 2020: A Survey of New Directions. Language Dynamics and Change. 2020b; 10 :3–26. doi: 10.1163/22105832-bja10005. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • O’Shannessy Carmel, Brown Connor. Reflexive and Reciprocal Encoding in the Australian Mixed Language, Light Warlpiri. Langages. 2021; 6 :105. doi: 10.3390/languages6020105. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • O’Shannessy Carmel. Light Warlpiri: A new language. Australian Journal of Linguistics. 2005; 25 :31–57. doi: 10.1080/07268600500110472. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • O’Shannessy Carmel. The role of codeswitched input to children in the origin of a new mixed language. Linguistics. 2012; 50 :305–40. doi: 10.1515/ling-2012-0011. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • O’Shannessy Carmel. The role of multiple sources in the formation of an innovative auxiliary category in Light Warlpiri, a new Australian mixed language. Language. 2013; 89 :328–53. doi: 10.1353/lan.2013.0025. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • O’Shannessy Carmel. How ordinary child language acquisition processes can lead to the unusual outcome of a mixed language. International Journal of Bilingualism. 2020; 25 :458–80. doi: 10.1177/1367006920924957. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Obler Loraine K., Gjerlow Kris. Language and the Brain. Cambridge University Press; Cambridge: 1999. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Olson David R. Intelligence and Technology. Routledge; London: 2005. Technology and intelligence in a literate society; pp. 79–92. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Olson David R. Writing: A Mosaic of New Perspectives. Psychology Press; New York: 2012. Language, literacy and mind: The literacy hypothesis; pp. 3–10. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Open Science Collaboration Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science. Science. 2015; 349 :aac4716. doi: 10.1126/science.aac4716. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Passmore Sam, Wood Anna L. C., Barbieri Chiara, Barbieri Chiara, Shilton Dor, Daikoku Hideo, Atkinson Quentin, Savage Patrick E. Global relationships between musical, linguistic, and genetic diversity. PsyArXiv. 2022 doi: 10.31234/osf.io/mdrsn. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pearl Judea. Causality: Models, Reasoning, and Inference. Cambridge University Press; Cambridge: 2000. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pérez-Losada Joaquim, Fort Joaquim. A serial founder effect model of phonemic diversity based on phonemic loss in low-density populations. PLoS ONE. 2018; 13 :e0198346. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0198346. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Perlman Marcus. Debunking two myths against vocal origins of language: Language is iconic and multimodal to the core. Interaction Studies. Social Behaviour and Communication in Biological and Artificial Systems. 2017; 18 :376–401. doi: 10.1075/is.18.3.05per. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Novak Petra Kralj, Smailović Jasmina, Sluban Borut, Mozetič Igor. Sentiment of Emojis. PLoS ONE. 2015; 10 :e0144296. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0144296. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pierini Francesco. Emojis and gestures: A new typology. Proceedings of Sinn Und Bedeutung. 2021; 25 :720–32. doi: 10.18148/sub/2021.v25i0.963. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pinker Steven, Bloom Paul. Natural language and natural selection. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 1990; 13 :707–27. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00081061. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pleyer Michael, Hartmann Stefen. Constructing a Consensus on Language Evolution? Convergences and Differences Between Biolinguistic and Usage-Based Approaches. Frontiers in Psychology. 2019; 10 :2537–37. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02537. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Preiss David D. Human Intelligence in the Time of the Anthropocene. In: Sternberg Robert J., Preiss David D., editors. Intelligence in Context: The Cultural and Historical Foundations of Human Intelligence. Springer International Publishing; Berlin: 2022. pp. 361–91. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Preiss David D., Sternberg Robert J. Effects of technology on verbal and visual-spatial abilities. International Journal of Cognitive Technology. 2006; 11 :14–22. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Prugnolle Franck, Manica Andrea, Balloux Francois. Geography predicts neutral genetic diversity of human populations. Current Biology. 2005; 15 :R159–R160. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2005.02.038. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ramachandran Sohini, Deshpande Omkar, Roseman Charles C., Rosenberg Noah A., Feldman Marcus W., Cavalli-Sforza Luigi Luca. Support from the relationship of genetic and geographic distance in human populations for a serial founder effect originating in Africa. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2005; 102 :15942–47. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0507611102. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rizzolatti Giacomo, Arbib Michael A. Language within our grasp. Trends in Neurosciences. 1998; 21 :188–94. doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(98)01260-0. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Roberts Sean G. Robust, Causal, and Incremental Approaches to Investigating Linguistic Adaptation. Frontiers in Psychology. 2018; 9 :166. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00166. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Roberts Seán G., Killin Anton, Deb Angarika, Sheard Catherine, Greenhill Simon J., Sinnemäki Kaius, Segovia-Martín José, Nölle Jonas, Berdicevskis Aleksandrs, Humphreys-Balkwill Archie. CHIELD: The causal hypotheses in evolutionary linguistics database. Journal of Language Evolution. 2020; 5 :101–20. doi: 10.1093/jole/lzaa001. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Roberts Sean, Winters James. Social Structure and Language Structure: The New Nomothetic Approach. Psychology of Language and Communication. 2012; 16 :89–112. doi: 10.2478/v10057-012-0008-6. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Robinson Elizabeth, Goelman Hillel, Olson David R. Children’s understanding of the relation between expressions (what was said) and intentions (what was meant) British Journal of Developmental Psychology. 1983; 1 :75–86. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-835X.1983.tb00545.x. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Tagliamonte Sali A., Denis Derek. Linguistic ruin? Lol! Instant messaging and teen language. American Speech. 2008; 83 :3–34. doi: 10.1215/00031283-2008-001. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Saussure Ferdinand de, Bally Charles, Sechehaye Albert, Riedlinger Albert. Course in General Linguistics. Open Court Publishing; LaSalle: 1986. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schmandt-Besserat Denise. Writing: A Mosaic of New Perspectives. Psychology Press; New York: 2012. Tokens as precursors of writing; pp. 3–10. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Semple Stuart, Hsu Minna J., Agoramoorthy Govindasamy. Efficiency of coding in macaque vocal communication. Biology Letters. 2010; 6 :469–71. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2009.1062. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Tomasello Michael. The usage-based theory of language acquisition. In: Bavin Edith L., editor. The Cambridge Handbook of Child Language. Cambridge University Press; Cambridge: 2009. pp. 69–88. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Varnhagen Connie K., McFall G. Peggy, Pugh Nicole, Routledge Lisa, Sumida-MacDonald Heather, Kwong Trudy E. lol: New language and spelling in instant messaging. Reading and Writing. 2010; 23 :719–33. doi: 10.1007/s11145-009-9181-y. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Webb Vic. Medium of Instruction Policies. Routledge; New York: 2003. Language Policy in Post-Apartheid South Africa. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wernicke Carl. A Psychological Study on an Anatomical Basis: The Aphasia Symptom-Complex. In: Eling Paul., editor. Reader in the History of Aphasia: From Franz Gall to Norman Geschwind. vol. 4. John Benjamins Publishing Company; Amsterdam: 1994. [(accessed on 17 March 2023)]. pp. 69–90. Classics in Psycholinguistics. First published 1874. Available online: https://benjamins.com/catalog/cipl.4.11the [ Google Scholar ]
  • Zipf George Kingsley. Human Behavior and the Principle of Least Effort. Wiley; Hoboken: 1949. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Zlatev Jordan, Wacewicz Sławomir, Zywiczynski Przemyslaw, Weijer Joost van de. Multimodal-first or pantomime-first? Communicating events through pantomime with and without vocalization. Interaction Studies. Social Behaviour and Communication in Biological and Artificial Systems. 2017; 18 :465–88. doi: 10.1075/is.18.3.08zla. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Search Menu

Sign in through your institution

  • Browse content in Arts and Humanities
  • Browse content in Archaeology
  • Anglo-Saxon and Medieval Archaeology
  • Archaeological Methodology and Techniques
  • Archaeology by Region
  • Archaeology of Religion
  • Archaeology of Trade and Exchange
  • Biblical Archaeology
  • Contemporary and Public Archaeology
  • Environmental Archaeology
  • Historical Archaeology
  • History and Theory of Archaeology
  • Industrial Archaeology
  • Landscape Archaeology
  • Mortuary Archaeology
  • Prehistoric Archaeology
  • Underwater Archaeology
  • Zooarchaeology
  • Browse content in Architecture
  • Architectural Structure and Design
  • History of Architecture
  • Residential and Domestic Buildings
  • Theory of Architecture
  • Browse content in Art
  • Art Subjects and Themes
  • History of Art
  • Industrial and Commercial Art
  • Theory of Art
  • Biographical Studies
  • Byzantine Studies
  • Browse content in Classical Studies
  • Classical History
  • Classical Philosophy
  • Classical Mythology
  • Classical Numismatics
  • Classical Literature
  • Classical Reception
  • Classical Art and Architecture
  • Classical Oratory and Rhetoric
  • Greek and Roman Epigraphy
  • Greek and Roman Law
  • Greek and Roman Papyrology
  • Greek and Roman Archaeology
  • Late Antiquity
  • Religion in the Ancient World
  • Social History
  • Digital Humanities
  • Browse content in History
  • Colonialism and Imperialism
  • Diplomatic History
  • Environmental History
  • Genealogy, Heraldry, Names, and Honours
  • Genocide and Ethnic Cleansing
  • Historical Geography
  • History by Period
  • History of Emotions
  • History of Agriculture
  • History of Education
  • History of Gender and Sexuality
  • Industrial History
  • Intellectual History
  • International History
  • Labour History
  • Legal and Constitutional History
  • Local and Family History
  • Maritime History
  • Military History
  • National Liberation and Post-Colonialism
  • Oral History
  • Political History
  • Public History
  • Regional and National History
  • Revolutions and Rebellions
  • Slavery and Abolition of Slavery
  • Social and Cultural History
  • Theory, Methods, and Historiography
  • Urban History
  • World History
  • Browse content in Language Teaching and Learning
  • Language Learning (Specific Skills)
  • Language Teaching Theory and Methods
  • Browse content in Linguistics
  • Applied Linguistics
  • Cognitive Linguistics
  • Computational Linguistics
  • Forensic Linguistics
  • Grammar, Syntax and Morphology
  • Historical and Diachronic Linguistics
  • History of English
  • Language Acquisition
  • Language Evolution
  • Language Reference
  • Language Variation
  • Language Families
  • Lexicography
  • Linguistic Anthropology
  • Linguistic Theories
  • Linguistic Typology
  • Phonetics and Phonology
  • Psycholinguistics
  • Sociolinguistics
  • Translation and Interpretation
  • Writing Systems
  • Browse content in Literature
  • Bibliography
  • Children's Literature Studies
  • Literary Studies (Asian)
  • Literary Studies (European)
  • Literary Studies (Eco-criticism)
  • Literary Studies (Romanticism)
  • Literary Studies (American)
  • Literary Studies (Modernism)
  • Literary Studies - World
  • Literary Studies (1500 to 1800)
  • Literary Studies (19th Century)
  • Literary Studies (20th Century onwards)
  • Literary Studies (African American Literature)
  • Literary Studies (British and Irish)
  • Literary Studies (Early and Medieval)
  • Literary Studies (Fiction, Novelists, and Prose Writers)
  • Literary Studies (Gender Studies)
  • Literary Studies (Graphic Novels)
  • Literary Studies (History of the Book)
  • Literary Studies (Plays and Playwrights)
  • Literary Studies (Poetry and Poets)
  • Literary Studies (Postcolonial Literature)
  • Literary Studies (Queer Studies)
  • Literary Studies (Science Fiction)
  • Literary Studies (Travel Literature)
  • Literary Studies (War Literature)
  • Literary Studies (Women's Writing)
  • Literary Theory and Cultural Studies
  • Mythology and Folklore
  • Shakespeare Studies and Criticism
  • Browse content in Media Studies
  • Browse content in Music
  • Applied Music
  • Dance and Music
  • Ethics in Music
  • Ethnomusicology
  • Gender and Sexuality in Music
  • Medicine and Music
  • Music Cultures
  • Music and Religion
  • Music and Media
  • Music and Culture
  • Music Education and Pedagogy
  • Music Theory and Analysis
  • Musical Scores, Lyrics, and Libretti
  • Musical Structures, Styles, and Techniques
  • Musicology and Music History
  • Performance Practice and Studies
  • Race and Ethnicity in Music
  • Sound Studies
  • Browse content in Performing Arts
  • Browse content in Philosophy
  • Aesthetics and Philosophy of Art
  • Epistemology
  • Feminist Philosophy
  • History of Western Philosophy
  • Metaphysics
  • Moral Philosophy
  • Non-Western Philosophy
  • Philosophy of Science
  • Philosophy of Language
  • Philosophy of Mind
  • Philosophy of Perception
  • Philosophy of Action
  • Philosophy of Law
  • Philosophy of Religion
  • Philosophy of Mathematics and Logic
  • Practical Ethics
  • Social and Political Philosophy
  • Browse content in Religion
  • Biblical Studies
  • Christianity
  • East Asian Religions
  • History of Religion
  • Judaism and Jewish Studies
  • Qumran Studies
  • Religion and Education
  • Religion and Health
  • Religion and Politics
  • Religion and Science
  • Religion and Law
  • Religion and Art, Literature, and Music
  • Religious Studies
  • Browse content in Society and Culture
  • Cookery, Food, and Drink
  • Cultural Studies
  • Customs and Traditions
  • Ethical Issues and Debates
  • Hobbies, Games, Arts and Crafts
  • Natural world, Country Life, and Pets
  • Popular Beliefs and Controversial Knowledge
  • Sports and Outdoor Recreation
  • Technology and Society
  • Travel and Holiday
  • Visual Culture
  • Browse content in Law
  • Arbitration
  • Browse content in Company and Commercial Law
  • Commercial Law
  • Company Law
  • Browse content in Comparative Law
  • Systems of Law
  • Competition Law
  • Browse content in Constitutional and Administrative Law
  • Government Powers
  • Judicial Review
  • Local Government Law
  • Military and Defence Law
  • Parliamentary and Legislative Practice
  • Construction Law
  • Contract Law
  • Browse content in Criminal Law
  • Criminal Procedure
  • Criminal Evidence Law
  • Sentencing and Punishment
  • Employment and Labour Law
  • Environment and Energy Law
  • Browse content in Financial Law
  • Banking Law
  • Insolvency Law
  • History of Law
  • Human Rights and Immigration
  • Intellectual Property Law
  • Browse content in International Law
  • Private International Law and Conflict of Laws
  • Public International Law
  • IT and Communications Law
  • Jurisprudence and Philosophy of Law
  • Law and Politics
  • Law and Society
  • Browse content in Legal System and Practice
  • Courts and Procedure
  • Legal Skills and Practice
  • Legal System - Costs and Funding
  • Primary Sources of Law
  • Regulation of Legal Profession
  • Medical and Healthcare Law
  • Browse content in Policing
  • Criminal Investigation and Detection
  • Police and Security Services
  • Police Procedure and Law
  • Police Regional Planning
  • Browse content in Property Law
  • Personal Property Law
  • Restitution
  • Study and Revision
  • Terrorism and National Security Law
  • Browse content in Trusts Law
  • Wills and Probate or Succession
  • Browse content in Medicine and Health
  • Browse content in Allied Health Professions
  • Arts Therapies
  • Clinical Science
  • Dietetics and Nutrition
  • Occupational Therapy
  • Operating Department Practice
  • Physiotherapy
  • Radiography
  • Speech and Language Therapy
  • Browse content in Anaesthetics
  • General Anaesthesia
  • Browse content in Clinical Medicine
  • Acute Medicine
  • Cardiovascular Medicine
  • Clinical Genetics
  • Clinical Pharmacology and Therapeutics
  • Dermatology
  • Endocrinology and Diabetes
  • Gastroenterology
  • Genito-urinary Medicine
  • Geriatric Medicine
  • Infectious Diseases
  • Medical Toxicology
  • Medical Oncology
  • Pain Medicine
  • Palliative Medicine
  • Rehabilitation Medicine
  • Respiratory Medicine and Pulmonology
  • Rheumatology
  • Sleep Medicine
  • Sports and Exercise Medicine
  • Clinical Neuroscience
  • Community Medical Services
  • Critical Care
  • Emergency Medicine
  • Forensic Medicine
  • Haematology
  • History of Medicine
  • Browse content in Medical Dentistry
  • Oral and Maxillofacial Surgery
  • Paediatric Dentistry
  • Restorative Dentistry and Orthodontics
  • Surgical Dentistry
  • Browse content in Medical Skills
  • Clinical Skills
  • Communication Skills
  • Nursing Skills
  • Surgical Skills
  • Medical Ethics
  • Medical Statistics and Methodology
  • Browse content in Neurology
  • Clinical Neurophysiology
  • Neuropathology
  • Nursing Studies
  • Browse content in Obstetrics and Gynaecology
  • Gynaecology
  • Occupational Medicine
  • Ophthalmology
  • Otolaryngology (ENT)
  • Browse content in Paediatrics
  • Neonatology
  • Browse content in Pathology
  • Chemical Pathology
  • Clinical Cytogenetics and Molecular Genetics
  • Histopathology
  • Medical Microbiology and Virology
  • Patient Education and Information
  • Browse content in Pharmacology
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Browse content in Popular Health
  • Caring for Others
  • Complementary and Alternative Medicine
  • Self-help and Personal Development
  • Browse content in Preclinical Medicine
  • Cell Biology
  • Molecular Biology and Genetics
  • Reproduction, Growth and Development
  • Primary Care
  • Professional Development in Medicine
  • Browse content in Psychiatry
  • Addiction Medicine
  • Child and Adolescent Psychiatry
  • Forensic Psychiatry
  • Learning Disabilities
  • Old Age Psychiatry
  • Psychotherapy
  • Browse content in Public Health and Epidemiology
  • Epidemiology
  • Public Health
  • Browse content in Radiology
  • Clinical Radiology
  • Interventional Radiology
  • Nuclear Medicine
  • Radiation Oncology
  • Reproductive Medicine
  • Browse content in Surgery
  • Cardiothoracic Surgery
  • Gastro-intestinal and Colorectal Surgery
  • General Surgery
  • Neurosurgery
  • Paediatric Surgery
  • Peri-operative Care
  • Plastic and Reconstructive Surgery
  • Surgical Oncology
  • Transplant Surgery
  • Trauma and Orthopaedic Surgery
  • Vascular Surgery
  • Browse content in Science and Mathematics
  • Browse content in Biological Sciences
  • Aquatic Biology
  • Biochemistry
  • Bioinformatics and Computational Biology
  • Developmental Biology
  • Ecology and Conservation
  • Evolutionary Biology
  • Genetics and Genomics
  • Microbiology
  • Molecular and Cell Biology
  • Natural History
  • Plant Sciences and Forestry
  • Research Methods in Life Sciences
  • Structural Biology
  • Systems Biology
  • Zoology and Animal Sciences
  • Browse content in Chemistry
  • Analytical Chemistry
  • Computational Chemistry
  • Crystallography
  • Environmental Chemistry
  • Industrial Chemistry
  • Inorganic Chemistry
  • Materials Chemistry
  • Medicinal Chemistry
  • Mineralogy and Gems
  • Organic Chemistry
  • Physical Chemistry
  • Polymer Chemistry
  • Study and Communication Skills in Chemistry
  • Theoretical Chemistry
  • Browse content in Computer Science
  • Artificial Intelligence
  • Computer Architecture and Logic Design
  • Game Studies
  • Human-Computer Interaction
  • Mathematical Theory of Computation
  • Programming Languages
  • Software Engineering
  • Systems Analysis and Design
  • Virtual Reality
  • Browse content in Computing
  • Business Applications
  • Computer Security
  • Computer Games
  • Computer Networking and Communications
  • Digital Lifestyle
  • Graphical and Digital Media Applications
  • Operating Systems
  • Browse content in Earth Sciences and Geography
  • Atmospheric Sciences
  • Environmental Geography
  • Geology and the Lithosphere
  • Maps and Map-making
  • Meteorology and Climatology
  • Oceanography and Hydrology
  • Palaeontology
  • Physical Geography and Topography
  • Regional Geography
  • Soil Science
  • Urban Geography
  • Browse content in Engineering and Technology
  • Agriculture and Farming
  • Biological Engineering
  • Civil Engineering, Surveying, and Building
  • Electronics and Communications Engineering
  • Energy Technology
  • Engineering (General)
  • Environmental Science, Engineering, and Technology
  • History of Engineering and Technology
  • Mechanical Engineering and Materials
  • Technology of Industrial Chemistry
  • Transport Technology and Trades
  • Browse content in Environmental Science
  • Applied Ecology (Environmental Science)
  • Conservation of the Environment (Environmental Science)
  • Environmental Sustainability
  • Environmentalist Thought and Ideology (Environmental Science)
  • Management of Land and Natural Resources (Environmental Science)
  • Natural Disasters (Environmental Science)
  • Nuclear Issues (Environmental Science)
  • Pollution and Threats to the Environment (Environmental Science)
  • Social Impact of Environmental Issues (Environmental Science)
  • History of Science and Technology
  • Browse content in Materials Science
  • Ceramics and Glasses
  • Composite Materials
  • Metals, Alloying, and Corrosion
  • Nanotechnology
  • Browse content in Mathematics
  • Applied Mathematics
  • Biomathematics and Statistics
  • History of Mathematics
  • Mathematical Education
  • Mathematical Finance
  • Mathematical Analysis
  • Numerical and Computational Mathematics
  • Probability and Statistics
  • Pure Mathematics
  • Browse content in Neuroscience
  • Cognition and Behavioural Neuroscience
  • Development of the Nervous System
  • Disorders of the Nervous System
  • History of Neuroscience
  • Invertebrate Neurobiology
  • Molecular and Cellular Systems
  • Neuroendocrinology and Autonomic Nervous System
  • Neuroscientific Techniques
  • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • Browse content in Physics
  • Astronomy and Astrophysics
  • Atomic, Molecular, and Optical Physics
  • Biological and Medical Physics
  • Classical Mechanics
  • Computational Physics
  • Condensed Matter Physics
  • Electromagnetism, Optics, and Acoustics
  • History of Physics
  • Mathematical and Statistical Physics
  • Measurement Science
  • Nuclear Physics
  • Particles and Fields
  • Plasma Physics
  • Quantum Physics
  • Relativity and Gravitation
  • Semiconductor and Mesoscopic Physics
  • Browse content in Psychology
  • Affective Sciences
  • Clinical Psychology
  • Cognitive Psychology
  • Cognitive Neuroscience
  • Criminal and Forensic Psychology
  • Developmental Psychology
  • Educational Psychology
  • Evolutionary Psychology
  • Health Psychology
  • History and Systems in Psychology
  • Music Psychology
  • Neuropsychology
  • Organizational Psychology
  • Psychological Assessment and Testing
  • Psychology of Human-Technology Interaction
  • Psychology Professional Development and Training
  • Research Methods in Psychology
  • Social Psychology
  • Browse content in Social Sciences
  • Browse content in Anthropology
  • Anthropology of Religion
  • Human Evolution
  • Medical Anthropology
  • Physical Anthropology
  • Regional Anthropology
  • Social and Cultural Anthropology
  • Theory and Practice of Anthropology
  • Browse content in Business and Management
  • Business Strategy
  • Business Ethics
  • Business History
  • Business and Government
  • Business and Technology
  • Business and the Environment
  • Comparative Management
  • Corporate Governance
  • Corporate Social Responsibility
  • Entrepreneurship
  • Health Management
  • Human Resource Management
  • Industrial and Employment Relations
  • Industry Studies
  • Information and Communication Technologies
  • International Business
  • Knowledge Management
  • Management and Management Techniques
  • Operations Management
  • Organizational Theory and Behaviour
  • Pensions and Pension Management
  • Public and Nonprofit Management
  • Social Issues in Business and Management
  • Strategic Management
  • Supply Chain Management
  • Browse content in Criminology and Criminal Justice
  • Criminal Justice
  • Criminology
  • Forms of Crime
  • International and Comparative Criminology
  • Youth Violence and Juvenile Justice
  • Development Studies
  • Browse content in Economics
  • Agricultural, Environmental, and Natural Resource Economics
  • Asian Economics
  • Behavioural Finance
  • Behavioural Economics and Neuroeconomics
  • Econometrics and Mathematical Economics
  • Economic Systems
  • Economic History
  • Economic Methodology
  • Economic Development and Growth
  • Financial Markets
  • Financial Institutions and Services
  • General Economics and Teaching
  • Health, Education, and Welfare
  • History of Economic Thought
  • International Economics
  • Labour and Demographic Economics
  • Law and Economics
  • Macroeconomics and Monetary Economics
  • Microeconomics
  • Public Economics
  • Urban, Rural, and Regional Economics
  • Welfare Economics
  • Browse content in Education
  • Adult Education and Continuous Learning
  • Care and Counselling of Students
  • Early Childhood and Elementary Education
  • Educational Equipment and Technology
  • Educational Strategies and Policy
  • Higher and Further Education
  • Organization and Management of Education
  • Philosophy and Theory of Education
  • Schools Studies
  • Secondary Education
  • Teaching of a Specific Subject
  • Teaching of Specific Groups and Special Educational Needs
  • Teaching Skills and Techniques
  • Browse content in Environment
  • Applied Ecology (Social Science)
  • Climate Change
  • Conservation of the Environment (Social Science)
  • Environmentalist Thought and Ideology (Social Science)
  • Management of Land and Natural Resources (Social Science)
  • Natural Disasters (Environment)
  • Pollution and Threats to the Environment (Social Science)
  • Social Impact of Environmental Issues (Social Science)
  • Sustainability
  • Browse content in Human Geography
  • Cultural Geography
  • Economic Geography
  • Political Geography
  • Browse content in Interdisciplinary Studies
  • Communication Studies
  • Museums, Libraries, and Information Sciences
  • Browse content in Politics
  • African Politics
  • Asian Politics
  • Chinese Politics
  • Comparative Politics
  • Conflict Politics
  • Elections and Electoral Studies
  • Environmental Politics
  • Ethnic Politics
  • European Union
  • Foreign Policy
  • Gender and Politics
  • Human Rights and Politics
  • Indian Politics
  • International Relations
  • International Organization (Politics)
  • Irish Politics
  • Latin American Politics
  • Middle Eastern Politics
  • Political Methodology
  • Political Communication
  • Political Philosophy
  • Political Sociology
  • Political Behaviour
  • Political Economy
  • Political Institutions
  • Political Theory
  • Politics and Law
  • Politics of Development
  • Public Administration
  • Public Policy
  • Qualitative Political Methodology
  • Quantitative Political Methodology
  • Regional Political Studies
  • Russian Politics
  • Security Studies
  • State and Local Government
  • UK Politics
  • US Politics
  • Browse content in Regional and Area Studies
  • African Studies
  • Asian Studies
  • East Asian Studies
  • Japanese Studies
  • Latin American Studies
  • Middle Eastern Studies
  • Native American Studies
  • Scottish Studies
  • Browse content in Research and Information
  • Research Methods
  • Browse content in Social Work
  • Addictions and Substance Misuse
  • Adoption and Fostering
  • Care of the Elderly
  • Child and Adolescent Social Work
  • Couple and Family Social Work
  • Direct Practice and Clinical Social Work
  • Emergency Services
  • Human Behaviour and the Social Environment
  • International and Global Issues in Social Work
  • Mental and Behavioural Health
  • Social Justice and Human Rights
  • Social Policy and Advocacy
  • Social Work and Crime and Justice
  • Social Work Macro Practice
  • Social Work Practice Settings
  • Social Work Research and Evidence-based Practice
  • Welfare and Benefit Systems
  • Browse content in Sociology
  • Childhood Studies
  • Community Development
  • Comparative and Historical Sociology
  • Disability Studies
  • Economic Sociology
  • Gender and Sexuality
  • Gerontology and Ageing
  • Health, Illness, and Medicine
  • Marriage and the Family
  • Migration Studies
  • Occupations, Professions, and Work
  • Organizations
  • Population and Demography
  • Race and Ethnicity
  • Social Theory
  • Social Movements and Social Change
  • Social Research and Statistics
  • Social Stratification, Inequality, and Mobility
  • Sociology of Religion
  • Sociology of Education
  • Sport and Leisure
  • Urban and Rural Studies
  • Browse content in Warfare and Defence
  • Defence Strategy, Planning, and Research
  • Land Forces and Warfare
  • Military Administration
  • Military Life and Institutions
  • Naval Forces and Warfare
  • Other Warfare and Defence Issues
  • Peace Studies and Conflict Resolution
  • Weapons and Equipment

The Oxford Handbook of the History of Linguistics

  • < Previous chapter
  • Next chapter >

1 The Origins and the Evolution of Language

Salikoko S. Mufwene is Frank J. McLoraine Distinguished Service Professor of Linguistics and the College at the University of Chicago, where he also serves as Professor on the Committee on Evolutionary Biology and on the Committee on the Conceptual and Historical Studies of Science. His current research is on language evolution, including the indigenization of English and other colonial European languages worldwide. He is the author of The Ecology of Language Evolution (Cambridge University Press 2001), Créoles , Écologie Sociale , Évolution Linguistique (l’Harmattan 2005), and Language Evolution: Contact, Competition and Change (Continuum Press 2008); and the (co-)editor of Africanisms in Afro-American Language Varieties (UGA Press 1993) and African American English: Structure, History and Use (Routledge 1998). He edits the book series Cambridge Approaches to Language Contact .

  • Published: 01 July 2013
  • Cite Icon Cite
  • Permissions Icon Permissions

This chapter is a selective comparative and critical survey of speculations/hypotheses, since Antiquity, on the phylogenetic emergence of language in mankind. It highlights topics and explanations that have been recurring, how some of them have been refined and/or enriched by modern thinking about hominin evolution since Charles Darwin’s account of the process by natural selection under specific ecological pressures. It also shows how some questions have been shaped by the manifold evolution of linguistics itself since the nineteenth century, including variation on what counts as language, and by intellectual exchanges between linguistics and other disciplines such as primatology, neurology, and paleontology. It concludes with an itemization of accomplishments, after articulating a long list of question-begging accounts and still unanswered questions.

Personal account

  • Sign in with email/username & password
  • Get email alerts
  • Save searches
  • Purchase content
  • Activate your purchase/trial code
  • Add your ORCID iD

Institutional access

Sign in with a library card.

  • Sign in with username/password
  • Recommend to your librarian
  • Institutional account management
  • Get help with access

Access to content on Oxford Academic is often provided through institutional subscriptions and purchases. If you are a member of an institution with an active account, you may be able to access content in one of the following ways:

IP based access

Typically, access is provided across an institutional network to a range of IP addresses. This authentication occurs automatically, and it is not possible to sign out of an IP authenticated account.

Choose this option to get remote access when outside your institution. Shibboleth/Open Athens technology is used to provide single sign-on between your institution’s website and Oxford Academic.

  • Click Sign in through your institution.
  • Select your institution from the list provided, which will take you to your institution's website to sign in.
  • When on the institution site, please use the credentials provided by your institution. Do not use an Oxford Academic personal account.
  • Following successful sign in, you will be returned to Oxford Academic.

If your institution is not listed or you cannot sign in to your institution’s website, please contact your librarian or administrator.

Enter your library card number to sign in. If you cannot sign in, please contact your librarian.

Society Members

Society member access to a journal is achieved in one of the following ways:

Sign in through society site

Many societies offer single sign-on between the society website and Oxford Academic. If you see ‘Sign in through society site’ in the sign in pane within a journal:

  • Click Sign in through society site.
  • When on the society site, please use the credentials provided by that society. Do not use an Oxford Academic personal account.

If you do not have a society account or have forgotten your username or password, please contact your society.

Sign in using a personal account

Some societies use Oxford Academic personal accounts to provide access to their members. See below.

A personal account can be used to get email alerts, save searches, purchase content, and activate subscriptions.

Some societies use Oxford Academic personal accounts to provide access to their members.

Viewing your signed in accounts

Click the account icon in the top right to:

  • View your signed in personal account and access account management features.
  • View the institutional accounts that are providing access.

Signed in but can't access content

Oxford Academic is home to a wide variety of products. The institutional subscription may not cover the content that you are trying to access. If you believe you should have access to that content, please contact your librarian.

For librarians and administrators, your personal account also provides access to institutional account management. Here you will find options to view and activate subscriptions, manage institutional settings and access options, access usage statistics, and more.

Our books are available by subscription or purchase to libraries and institutions.

Month: Total Views:
October 2022 8
November 2022 5
December 2022 8
January 2023 8
February 2023 24
March 2023 18
April 2023 4
May 2023 8
June 2023 3
July 2023 7
August 2023 7
September 2023 16
October 2023 13
November 2023 12
December 2023 5
January 2024 14
February 2024 14
March 2024 7
April 2024 9
May 2024 6
June 2024 4
July 2024 5
August 2024 11
  • About Oxford Academic
  • Publish journals with us
  • University press partners
  • What we publish
  • New features  
  • Open access
  • Rights and permissions
  • Accessibility
  • Advertising
  • Media enquiries
  • Oxford University Press
  • Oxford Languages
  • University of Oxford

Oxford University Press is a department of the University of Oxford. It furthers the University's objective of excellence in research, scholarship, and education by publishing worldwide

  • Copyright © 2024 Oxford University Press
  • Cookie settings
  • Cookie policy
  • Privacy policy
  • Legal notice

This Feature Is Available To Subscribers Only

Sign In or Create an Account

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

For full access to this pdf, sign in to an existing account, or purchase an annual subscription.

We’re fighting to restore access to 500,000+ books in court this week. Join us!

Internet Archive Audio

dissertation language origin

  • This Just In
  • Grateful Dead
  • Old Time Radio
  • 78 RPMs and Cylinder Recordings
  • Audio Books & Poetry
  • Computers, Technology and Science
  • Music, Arts & Culture
  • News & Public Affairs
  • Spirituality & Religion
  • Radio News Archive

dissertation language origin

  • Flickr Commons
  • Occupy Wall Street Flickr
  • NASA Images
  • Solar System Collection
  • Ames Research Center

dissertation language origin

  • All Software
  • Old School Emulation
  • MS-DOS Games
  • Historical Software
  • Classic PC Games
  • Software Library
  • Kodi Archive and Support File
  • Vintage Software
  • CD-ROM Software
  • CD-ROM Software Library
  • Software Sites
  • Tucows Software Library
  • Shareware CD-ROMs
  • Software Capsules Compilation
  • CD-ROM Images
  • ZX Spectrum
  • DOOM Level CD

dissertation language origin

  • Smithsonian Libraries
  • FEDLINK (US)
  • Lincoln Collection
  • American Libraries
  • Canadian Libraries
  • Universal Library
  • Project Gutenberg
  • Children's Library
  • Biodiversity Heritage Library
  • Books by Language
  • Additional Collections

dissertation language origin

  • Prelinger Archives
  • Democracy Now!
  • Occupy Wall Street
  • TV NSA Clip Library
  • Animation & Cartoons
  • Arts & Music
  • Computers & Technology
  • Cultural & Academic Films
  • Ephemeral Films
  • Sports Videos
  • Videogame Videos
  • Youth Media

Search the history of over 866 billion web pages on the Internet.

Mobile Apps

  • Wayback Machine (iOS)
  • Wayback Machine (Android)

Browser Extensions

Archive-it subscription.

  • Explore the Collections
  • Build Collections

Save Page Now

Capture a web page as it appears now for use as a trusted citation in the future.

Please enter a valid web address

  • Donate Donate icon An illustration of a heart shape

The origin and diversification of language

Bookreader item preview, share or embed this item, flag this item for.

  • Graphic Violence
  • Explicit Sexual Content
  • Hate Speech
  • Misinformation/Disinformation
  • Marketing/Phishing/Advertising
  • Misleading/Inaccurate/Missing Metadata

[WorldCat (this item)]

plus-circle Add Review comment Reviews

2 Favorites

DOWNLOAD OPTIONS

No suitable files to display here.

IN COLLECTIONS

Uploaded by station22.cebu on June 30, 2023

SIMILAR ITEMS (based on metadata)

Information

  • Author Services

Initiatives

You are accessing a machine-readable page. In order to be human-readable, please install an RSS reader.

All articles published by MDPI are made immediately available worldwide under an open access license. No special permission is required to reuse all or part of the article published by MDPI, including figures and tables. For articles published under an open access Creative Common CC BY license, any part of the article may be reused without permission provided that the original article is clearly cited. For more information, please refer to https://www.mdpi.com/openaccess .

Feature papers represent the most advanced research with significant potential for high impact in the field. A Feature Paper should be a substantial original Article that involves several techniques or approaches, provides an outlook for future research directions and describes possible research applications.

Feature papers are submitted upon individual invitation or recommendation by the scientific editors and must receive positive feedback from the reviewers.

Editor’s Choice articles are based on recommendations by the scientific editors of MDPI journals from around the world. Editors select a small number of articles recently published in the journal that they believe will be particularly interesting to readers, or important in the respective research area. The aim is to provide a snapshot of some of the most exciting work published in the various research areas of the journal.

Original Submission Date Received: .

  • Active Journals
  • Find a Journal
  • Proceedings Series
  • For Authors
  • For Reviewers
  • For Editors
  • For Librarians
  • For Publishers
  • For Societies
  • For Conference Organizers
  • Open Access Policy
  • Institutional Open Access Program
  • Special Issues Guidelines
  • Editorial Process
  • Research and Publication Ethics
  • Article Processing Charges
  • Testimonials
  • Preprints.org
  • SciProfiles
  • Encyclopedia

jintelligence-logo

Article Menu

  • Subscribe SciFeed
  • Recommended Articles
  • PubMed/Medline
  • Google Scholar
  • on Google Scholar
  • Table of Contents

Find support for a specific problem in the support section of our website.

Please let us know what you think of our products and services.

Visit our dedicated information section to learn more about MDPI.

JSmol Viewer

Language: its origin and ongoing evolution.

dissertation language origin

1. Introduction

2. origin of language, 2.1. modality of origin, 2.2. origin of languages, 2.3. neural correlates of language, 3. language adaptation, 3.1. ecological adaptations, 3.2. socio-demographic adaptations, 3.3. technological adaptations, 4. conclusions, author contributions, institutional review board statement, informed consent statement, data availability statement, conflicts of interest.

  • Alemi, Razieh, Seyed Amir Hossein Batouli, Ebrahim Behzad, Mitra Ebrahimpoor, and Mohammad Ali Oghabian. 2018. Not single brain areas but a network is involved in language: Applications in presurgical planning. Clinical Neurology and Neurosurgery 165: 116–28. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Aliannejadi, Mohammad, Monica Landoni, Theo Huibers, Emiliana Murgia, and Maria Soledad Pera. 2021. Children’s Perspective on How Emojis Help Them to Recognise Relevant Results: Do Actions Speak Louder Than Words? Paper presented at 2021 Conference on Human Information Interaction and Retrieval, Canberra, Australia, March 19; pp. 301–5. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Al-Sharqi, Laila, and Irum Saeed Abbasi. 2020. The Influence of Technology on English Language and Literature. English Language Teaching 13: 7. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Altmann, Gabriel. 1980. Prolegomena to Menzerath’s law. Glottometrika 2: 1–10. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Anthropocene Working Group. 2019. What Is the Anthropocene?—Current Definition and Status. Available online: http://quaternary.stratigraphy.org/working-groups/anthropocene/ (accessed on 16 March 2023).
  • Arbib, Michael A. 2012. How the Brain Got Language: The Mirror System Hypothesis . Oxford: Oxford University Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ardila, Alfredo, Byron Bernal, and Monica Rosselli. 2016. How Localized are Language Brain Areas? A Review of Brodmann Areas Involvement in Oral Language. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 31: 112–22. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Ardila, Alfredo. 2011. There are two different language systems in the brain. Journal of Behavioral and Brain Science 1: 23. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ Green Version ]
  • Ardila, Alfredo. 2012. Interaction between lexical and grammatical language systems in the brain. Physics of Life Reviews 9: 198–214. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Atkinson, Quentin D. 2011. Phonemic Diversity Supports a Serial Founder Effect Model of Language Expansion from Africa. Science 332: 346–49. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ Green Version ]
  • Baldo, Juliana V., and Nina F. Dronkers. 2007. Neural correlates of arithmetic and language comprehension: A common substrate? Neuropsychologia 45: 229–35. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Barach, Elisa, Laurie Beth Feldman, and Heather Sheridan. 2021. Are emojis processed like words? Eye movements reveal the time course of semantic processing for emojified text. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 28: 978–91. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Barbieri, Chiara, Damián E. Blasi, Epifanía Arango-Isaza, Alexandros G. Sotiropoulos, Harald Hammarström, Søren Wichmann, Simon J. Greenhill, Russell D. Gray, Robert Forkel, Balthasar Bickel, and et al. 2022. A global analysis of matches and mismatches between human genetic and linguistic histories. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 119: e2122084119. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Bastiaansen, Marcel, and Peter Hagoort. 2006. Oscillatory neuronal dynamics during language comprehension. In Progress in Brain Research . Edited by Christa Neuper and Wolfgang Klimesch. Amsterdam: Elsevier, vol. 159, pp. 179–96. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ Green Version ]
  • Bentz, Christian, and Ramon Ferrer-i-Cancho. 2016. Zipf’s Law of Abbreviation as a Language Universal . Tübingen: University of Tübingen, pp. 1–4. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Bentz, Christian, Dan Dediu, Annemarie Verkerk, and Gerhard Jäger. 2018. The evolution of language families is shaped by the environment beyond neutral drift. Nature Human Behaviour 2: 11. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ] [ Green Version ]
  • Bezerra, Bruna M., Antonio S. Souto, Andrew N. Radford, and Gareth Jones. 2011. Brevity Is Not Always a Virtue in Primate Communication. Biology Letters 7: 23–25. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Boeckx, Cedric. 2021. Reflections on language evolution. In Language Science Press . Berlin: Language Science Press. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Bolhuis, Johan J., Ian Tattersall, Noam Chomsky, and Robert C. Berwick. 2014. How Could Language Have Evolved? PLoS Biology 12: e1001934. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ] [ Green Version ]
  • Bouckaert, Remco R., Philippe Lemey, Michael D. Dunn, Simon J. Greenhill, Alexander V. Alekseyenko, Alexei J. Drummond, Russell D. Gray, Marc A. Suchard, and Quentin D. Atkinson. 2012. Mapping the origins and expansion of the Indo-European language family. Science 337: 957–60. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ Green Version ]
  • Broca, Paul. 1861. Remarks on the seat of the faculty of articulated language, following an observation of aphemia (loss of speech). Bulletin de La Société Anatomique 6: 330–57. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bromham, Lindell, Xia Hua, Thomas G. Fitzpatrick, and Simon J. Greenhill. 2015. Rate of language evolution is affected by population size. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 112: 2097–102. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ] [ Green Version ]
  • Brown, Angela M., and Delvin T. Lindsey. 2004. Color and language: Worldwide distribution of Daltonism and distinct words for “blue”. Visual Neuroscience 21: 409–12. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Busnel, Rene-Guy, and André Classe. 1976. Whistled Languages . Berlin and Heidelberg: Springer. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Chomsky, Noam. 1988. Language and Problems of Knowledge: The Managua Lectures . Cambridge: MIT Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Christiansen, Morten H., and Nick Chater. 2016. The Now-or-Never bottleneck: A fundamental constraint on language. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 39: e62. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ] [ Green Version ]
  • Conradie, Jac, and Gerald Groenewald. 2014. Die ontstaan en vestiging van Afrikaans . Edited by W. A. M. Carstens and N. Bosman. Kontemporêre Afrikaanse Taalkunde. Pretoria: Van Schaik, pp. 27–60. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Corballis, Michael C. 2011. The Origins of Language in Manual Gestures . Oxford: Oxford University Press. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Corral, Álvaro, Isabel Serra, and Ramon Ferrer-i-Cancho. 2019. Distinct flavors of Zipf’s law and its maximum likelihood fitting: Rank-size and size-distribution representations. ArXiv: Data Analysis, Statistics and Probability 102: 052113. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Creanza, Nicole, Merritt Ruhlen, Trevor J. Pemberton, Noah A. Rosenberg, Marcus W. Feldman, and Sohini Ramachandran. 2015. A comparison of worldwide phonemic and genetic variation in human populations. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 112: 1265–72. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ Green Version ]
  • Dale, Rick, and Gary Lupyan. 2012. Understanding the origins of morphological diversity: The linguistic niche hypothesis. Advances in Complex Systems 15: 1150017. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ Green Version ]
  • Darwin, Charles. 1860. On the Origin of Species by Means of Natural Selection: Or the Preservation of the Favoured Races in the Struggle for Life . London: John Murray. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ Green Version ]
  • Darwin, Charles. 1871. The Descent of Man, and Selection in Relation to Sex, Vol. 1 . London: John Murray. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ Green Version ]
  • De Villiers, Johan. 2012. Die Nederlandse era aan die Kaap, 1652–1806. In Geskiedenis van Suid-Afrika. Van Voortye Tot Vandag Bl . Edited by Fransjohan Pretorius. Cape Town: Tafelberg Publishers Ltd., pp. 39–62. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dediu, Dan, and Bart de Boer. 2016. Language evolution needs its own journal. Journal of Language Evolution 1: 1–6. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Dediu, Dan, Rick Janssen, and Scott R. Moisik. 2017. Language is not isolated from its wider environment: Vocal tract influences on the evolution of speech and language. Language & Communication 54: 9–20. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ Green Version ]
  • Derrida, Jacques. 1976. Of Grammatology, Trans. Gayatri Chakravorty Spivak . Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Diewald, Gabriele. 2011. Pragmaticalization (Defined) as Grammaticalization of Discourse Functions . Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton, vol. 49, pp. 365–90. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ Green Version ]
  • Donohue, Mark, and Tim Denham. 2010. Farming and Language in Island Southeast Asia Reframing Austronesian History. Current Anthropology 51: 223–56. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ Green Version ]
  • Dronkers, Nina F. 1996. A new brain region for coordinating speech articulation. Nature 384: 159–61. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Dunbar, Robin I. M. 1997. Groups, Gossip, and the Evolution of Language. In New Aspects of Human Ethology . Edited by Alain Schmitt, Klaus Atzwanger, Karl Grammer and Katrin Schäfer. New York: Springer, pp. 77–89. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Erle, Thorsten M., Karoline Schmid, Simon H. Goslar, and Jared D. Martin. 2021. Emojis as social information in digital communication. Emotion 22: 1529–1543. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Everett, Caleb, Damián E. Blasi, and Seán G. Roberts. 2015. Climate, vocal folds, and tonal languages: Connecting the physiological and geographic dots. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 112: 1322–27. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ Green Version ]
  • Everett, Caleb. 2017. Languages in Drier Climates Use Fewer Vowels. Frontiers in Psychology 8: 1285. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ Green Version ]
  • Favaro, Livio, Marco Gamba, Eleonora Cresta, Elena Fumagalli, Francesca Bandoli, Cristina Pilenga, Valentina Isaja, Nicolas Mathevon, and David Reby. 2020. Do penguins’ vocal sequences conform to linguistic laws? Biology Letters 16: 20190589. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ] [ Green Version ]
  • Feldman, Laurie, Eliza Barach, Vidhushini Srinivasan, and Samira Shaikh. 2021. Emojis and Words Work Together in the Service of Communication . Palo Alto: Association for the Advancement of Artificial Intelligence. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Fitch, William T. 2010. The Evolution of Language . Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Formigari, Lia. 2013. L’origine del linguaggio. Ricognizioni storiche e valenze epistemologiche. In Sull’origine del Linguaggio e delle lingue storico-naturali. Un confronto tra linguisti e non linguisti . Edited by B. Emanuele. Roma: Bulzoni, pp. 13–22. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Goldman, Eric. 2018. Emojis and the Law. Washington Law Review 93: 1227. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Goody, Jack, and Ian Watt. 1963. The Consequences of Literacy. Comparative Studies in Society and History 5: 304–45. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ Green Version ]
  • Goody, Jack. 1986. The Logic of Writing and the Organization of Society , 1st ed. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press . [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Graffi, Giorgio. 2019. Origin of language and origin of languages. Evolutionary Linguistic Theory 1: 6–23. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Gray, Russell D., Alexei J. Drummond, and Simon J. Greenhill. 2009. Language Phylogenies Reveal Expansion Pulses and Pauses in Pacific Settlement. Science 323: 479–83. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ Green Version ]
  • Gray, Russell D., and Quentin D. Atkinson. 2003. Language-tree divergence times support the Anatolian theory of Indo-European origin. Nature 426: 435–39. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Grigorenko, Elena. 2023. The Never-Ending Innovativeness of the Wise Man. In Innovation, Creativity and Change Across Cultures . Edited by David D. Preiss, James C. Kaufman and Marcos Singer. London: Palgrave-Macmillan. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hagoort, Peter. 2005. On Broca, brain, and binding: A new framework. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9: 416–23. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ] [ Green Version ]
  • Hamans, Camiel. 2021. Afrikaans: A language where ideology and linguistics meet. Scripta Neophilologica Posnaniensia 21: 15–92. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Hauser, Mark D., Noam Chomsky, and William T. Fitch. 2002. The faculty of language: What is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? Science 298: 1569–79. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Jackson, Joshua Conrad, Kristen Lindquist, Ryan Drabble, Quentin Atkinson, and Joseph Watts. 2023. Valence-dependent mutation in lexical evolution. Nature Human Behaviour 7: 190–99. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Winters, James, Simon Kirby, and Kenny Smith. 2015. Languages adapt to their contextual niche. Language and Cognition 7: 415–49. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ Green Version ]
  • Jordan, Kirsten, H. -J. Heinze, Kai Lutz, Martin Kanowski, and Lutz Jäncke. 2001. Cortical activations during the mental rotation of different visual objects. Neuroimage 13: 143–52. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Koplenig, Alexander. 2019. Language structure is influenced by the number of speakers but seemingly not by the proportion of non-native speakers. Royal Society Open Science 6: 181274. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ Green Version ]
  • Labov, William. 1991. Sociolinguistic Patterns . Philadelphia: University of Philadelphia Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lančarič, Daniel. 2016. Sentential Acronyms in Informal Online Communication. Reviewed Conference Proceedings from an International Scientific Conference. pp. 12–17. Available online: https://faj.euba.sk/www_write/files/veda-vyskum/konferencie/zborniky/Cudzie_jazyky_v_premenach_casu_7_2016.pdf#page=10 (accessed on 2 December 2022).
  • Lee, Jungwoo, Cheong Kim, and Kun Chang Lee. 2021. Investigating the Negative Effects of Emojis in Facebook Sponsored Ads for Establishing Sustainable Marketing in Social Media. Sustainability 13: 4864. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Levinson, Stephen C., and Judith Holler. 2014. The origin of human multi-modal communication. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 369: 20130302. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ Green Version ]
  • Lewis, Molly, and Michael C. Frank. 2016. Linguistic niches emerge from pressures at multiple timescales. Cognitive Science . pp. 1385–90. Available online: https://langcog.stanford.edu/papers_new/lewis-2016-cogsci.pdf (accessed on 25 November 2022).
  • Lewis, Paul M. 2009. Ethnologue: Languages of the World . New York: Linguistic Society of America. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Lindsey, Delvin T., and Angela M. Brown. 2002. Color Naming and the Phototoxic Effects of Sunlight on the Eye. Psychological Science 13: 506–12. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Linell, Per. 2004. The Written Language Bias in Linguistics: Its Nature, Origins and Transformations . London: Routledge. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lock, Andy, and Matt Gers. 2012. The cultural evolution of written language and its effects: A Darwinian process from prehistory to the modern day. In Writing: A Mosaic of New Perspectives . New York: Psychology Press, pp. 11–35. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lupyan, Gary, and Rick Dale. 2010. Language Structure Is Partly Determined by Social Structure. PLoS ONE 5: e8559. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ] [ Green Version ]
  • Lupyan, Gary, and Rick Dale. 2016. Why Are There Different Languages? The Role of Adaptation in Linguistic Diversity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 20: 649–60. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Mace, Ruth, and Clare Holden. 2005. A phylogenetic approach to cultural evolution. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 20: 116–21. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • MacNeilage, Peter F. 2008. The Origin of Speech . Oxford: Oxford University Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Maddieson, I. 2018. Language Adapts to Environment: Sonority and Temperature. Frontiers in Communication 3: 28. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ Green Version ]
  • Maddieson, Ian, and Christophe Coupé. 2015. Human spoken language diversity and the acoustic adaptation hypothesis. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 138: 1838–38. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Maess, Burkhard, Stefan Koelsch, Thomas C Gunter, and Angela D Friederici. 2001. Musical syntax is processed in Broca’s area: An MEG study. Nature Neuroscience 4: 540–45. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • McLuhan, Marshall. 1962. The Gutenberg Galaxy: The Making of Typographic Man . Toronto: University of Toronto Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • McWhorter, John. 2013. John McWhorter: Txtng is killing language. JK!!!|TED Talk. Available online: https://www.ted.com/talks/john_mcwhorter_txtng_is_killing_language_jk (accessed on 28 November 2022).
  • Mendívil-Giró, José-Luis. 2018. Why Don’t Languages Adapt to Their Environment? Frontiers in Communication 3: 24. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ Green Version ]
  • Meyer, Julien, Marcelo O. Magnasco, and Diana Reiss. 2021. The Relevance of Human Whistled Languages for the Analysis and Decoding of Dolphin Communication. Frontiers in Psychology 12: 689501. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Meyer, Julien. 2015. Whistled Languages . Berlin: Springer. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Meyer, Julien. 2018. Revitalization of Whistled Languages. In The Routledge Handbook of Language Revitalization . London: Routledge, p. 552. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Miton, Helen, and Olivier Morin. 2019. When iconicity stands in the way of abbreviation: No Zipfian effect for figurative signals. PLoS ONE 14: e0220793. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ] [ Green Version ]
  • Nettle, Daniel, and Suzanne Romaine. 2000. Vanishing Voices: The Extinction of the World’s Languages . Oxford: Oxford University Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Nölle, Jonas, Riccardo Fusaroli, Gregory J. Mills, and Kristian Tylén. 2020a. Language as shaped by the environment: Linguistic construal in a collaborative spatial task. Palgrave Communications 6: 27. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ Green Version ]
  • Nölle, Jonas, Stefan Hartmann, and Peeter Tinits. 2020b. Language Evolution Research in the Year 2020: A Survey of New Directions. Language Dynamics and Change 10: 3–26. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • O’Shannessy, Carmel, and Connor Brown. 2021. Reflexive and Reciprocal Encoding in the Australian Mixed Language, Light Warlpiri. Langages 6: 105. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • O’Shannessy, Carmel. 2005. Light Warlpiri: A new language. Australian Journal of Linguistics 25: 31–57. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • O’Shannessy, Carmel. 2012. The role of codeswitched input to children in the origin of a new mixed language. Linguistics 50: 305–40. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • O’Shannessy, Carmel. 2013. The role of multiple sources in the formation of an innovative auxiliary category in Light Warlpiri, a new Australian mixed language. Language 89: 328–53. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • O’Shannessy, Carmel. 2020. How ordinary child language acquisition processes can lead to the unusual outcome of a mixed language. International Journal of Bilingualism 25: 458–80. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Obler, Loraine K., and Kris Gjerlow. 1999. Language and the Brain . Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Olson, David R. 2005. Technology and intelligence in a literate society. In Intelligence and Technology . London: Routledge, pp. 79–92. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Olson, David R. 2012. Language, literacy and mind: The literacy hypothesis. In Writing: A Mosaic of New Perspectives . New York: Psychology Press, pp. 3–10. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Open Science Collaboration. 2015. Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science. Science 349: aac4716. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ] [ Green Version ]
  • Passmore, Sam, Anna L. C. Wood, Chiara Barbieri, Chiara Barbieri, Dor Shilton, Hideo Daikoku, Quentin Atkinson, and Patrick E. Savage. 2022. Global relationships between musical, linguistic, and genetic diversity. PsyArXiv . [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Pearl, Judea. 2000. Causality: Models, Reasoning, and Inference . Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pérez-Losada, Joaquim, and Joaquim Fort. 2018. A serial founder effect model of phonemic diversity based on phonemic loss in low-density populations. PLoS ONE 13: e0198346. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Perlman, Marcus. 2017. Debunking two myths against vocal origins of language: Language is iconic and multimodal to the core. Interaction Studies. Social Behaviour and Communication in Biological and Artificial Systems 18: 376–401. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Novak, Petra Kralj, Jasmina Smailović, Borut Sluban, and Igor Mozetič. 2015. Sentiment of Emojis. PLoS ONE 10: e0144296. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ Green Version ]
  • Pierini, Francesco. 2021. Emojis and gestures: A new typology. Proceedings of Sinn Und Bedeutung 25: 720–32. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Pinker, Steven, and Paul Bloom. 1990. Natural language and natural selection. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13: 707–27. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ Green Version ]
  • Pleyer, Michael, and Stefen Hartmann. 2019. Constructing a Consensus on Language Evolution? Convergences and Differences Between Biolinguistic and Usage-Based Approaches. Frontiers in Psychology 10: 2537–37. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ] [ Green Version ]
  • Preiss, David D. 2022. Human Intelligence in the Time of the Anthropocene. In Intelligence in Context: The Cultural and Historical Foundations of Human Intelligence . Edited by Robert J. Sternberg and David D. Preiss. Berlin: Springer International Publishing, pp. 361–91. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Preiss, David D., and Robert J. Sternberg. 2006. Effects of technology on verbal and visual-spatial abilities. International Journal of Cognitive Technology 11: 14–22. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Prugnolle, Franck, Andrea Manica, and Francois Balloux. 2005. Geography predicts neutral genetic diversity of human populations. Current Biology 15: R159–R160. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ Green Version ]
  • Ramachandran, Sohini, Omkar Deshpande, Charles C. Roseman, Noah A. Rosenberg, Marcus W. Feldman, and Luigi Luca Cavalli-Sforza. 2005. Support from the relationship of genetic and geographic distance in human populations for a serial founder effect originating in Africa. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 102: 15942–47. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ Green Version ]
  • Rizzolatti, Giacomo, and Michael A. Arbib. 1998. Language within our grasp. Trends in Neurosciences 21: 188–94. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ] [ Green Version ]
  • Roberts, Sean G. 2018. Robust, Causal, and Incremental Approaches to Investigating Linguistic Adaptation. Frontiers in Psychology 9: 166. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ Green Version ]
  • Roberts, Seán G., Anton Killin, Angarika Deb, Catherine Sheard, Simon J. Greenhill, Kaius Sinnemäki, José Segovia-Martín, Jonas Nölle, Aleksandrs Berdicevskis, and Archie Humphreys-Balkwill. 2020. CHIELD: The causal hypotheses in evolutionary linguistics database. Journal of Language Evolution 5: 101–20. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Roberts, Sean, and James Winters. 2012. Social Structure and Language Structure: The New Nomothetic Approach. Psychology of Language and Communication 16: 89–112. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ Green Version ]
  • Robinson, Elizabeth, Hillel Goelman, and David R. Olson. 1983. Children’s understanding of the relation between expressions (what was said) and intentions (what was meant). British Journal of Developmental Psychology 1: 75–86. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Tagliamonte, Sali A., and Derek Denis. 2008. Linguistic ruin? Lol! Instant messaging and teen language. American Speech 83: 3–34. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ Green Version ]
  • Saussure, Ferdinand de, Charles Bally, Albert Sechehaye, and Albert Riedlinger. 1986. Course in General Linguistics . LaSalle: Open Court Publishing. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schmandt-Besserat, Denise. 2012. Tokens as precursors of writing. In Writing: A Mosaic of New Perspectives . New York: Psychology Press, pp. 3–10. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Semple, Stuart, Minna J. Hsu, and Govindasamy Agoramoorthy. 2010. Efficiency of coding in macaque vocal communication. Biology Letters 6: 469–71. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ Green Version ]
  • Tomasello, Michael. 2009. The usage-based theory of language acquisition. In The Cambridge Handbook of Child Language . Edited by Edith L. Bavin. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 69–88. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ Green Version ]
  • Varnhagen, Connie K., G. Peggy McFall, Nicole Pugh, Lisa Routledge, Heather Sumida-MacDonald, and Trudy E. Kwong. 2010. lol: New language and spelling in instant messaging. Reading and Writing 23: 719–33. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ Green Version ]
  • Webb, Vic. 2003. Language Policy in Post-Apartheid South Africa. In Medium of Instruction Policies . New York: Routledge. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wernicke, Carl. 1994. A Psychological Study on an Anatomical Basis: The Aphasia Symptom-Complex. In Reader in the History of Aphasia: From Franz Gall to Norman Geschwind . Edited by Paul Eling. Classics in Psycholinguistics. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company, vol. 4, pp. 69–90. First published 1874. Available online: https://benjamins.com/catalog/cipl.4.11the (accessed on 17 March 2023).
  • Zipf, George Kingsley. 1949. Human Behavior and the Principle of Least Effort . Hoboken: Wiley. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Zlatev, Jordan, Sławomir Wacewicz, Przemyslaw Zywiczynski, and Joost van de Weijer. 2017. Multimodal-first or pantomime-first? Communicating events through pantomime with and without vocalization. Interaction Studies. Social Behaviour and Communication in Biological and Artificial Systems 18: 465–88. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

Markov, I.; Kharitonova, K.; Grigorenko, E.L. Language: Its Origin and Ongoing Evolution. J. Intell. 2023 , 11 , 61. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence11040061

Markov I, Kharitonova K, Grigorenko EL. Language: Its Origin and Ongoing Evolution. Journal of Intelligence . 2023; 11(4):61. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence11040061

Markov, Ilia, Kseniia Kharitonova, and Elena L. Grigorenko. 2023. "Language: Its Origin and Ongoing Evolution" Journal of Intelligence 11, no. 4: 61. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence11040061

Article Metrics

Article access statistics, further information, mdpi initiatives, follow mdpi.

MDPI

Subscribe to receive issue release notifications and newsletters from MDPI journals

  • Advanced Search
  • All new items
  • Journal articles
  • Manuscripts
  • All Categories
  • Metaphysics and Epistemology
  • Epistemology
  • Metaphilosophy
  • Metaphysics
  • Philosophy of Action
  • Philosophy of Language
  • Philosophy of Mind
  • Philosophy of Religion
  • Value Theory
  • Applied Ethics
  • Meta-Ethics
  • Normative Ethics
  • Philosophy of Gender, Race, and Sexuality
  • Philosophy of Law
  • Social and Political Philosophy
  • Value Theory, Miscellaneous
  • Science, Logic, and Mathematics
  • Logic and Philosophy of Logic
  • Philosophy of Biology
  • Philosophy of Cognitive Science
  • Philosophy of Computing and Information
  • Philosophy of Mathematics
  • Philosophy of Physical Science
  • Philosophy of Social Science
  • Philosophy of Probability
  • General Philosophy of Science
  • Philosophy of Science, Misc
  • History of Western Philosophy
  • Ancient Greek and Roman Philosophy
  • Medieval and Renaissance Philosophy
  • 17th/18th Century Philosophy
  • 19th Century Philosophy
  • 20th Century Philosophy
  • History of Western Philosophy, Misc
  • Philosophical Traditions
  • African/Africana Philosophy
  • Asian Philosophy
  • Continental Philosophy
  • European Philosophy
  • Philosophy of the Americas
  • Philosophical Traditions, Miscellaneous
  • Philosophy, Misc
  • Philosophy, Introductions and Anthologies
  • Philosophy, General Works
  • Teaching Philosophy
  • Philosophy, Miscellaneous
  • Other Academic Areas
  • Natural Sciences
  • Social Sciences
  • Cognitive Sciences
  • Formal Sciences
  • Arts and Humanities
  • Professional Areas
  • Other Academic Areas, Misc
  • Submit a book or article
  • Upload a bibliography
  • Personal page tracking
  • Archives we track
  • Information for publishers
  • Introduction
  • Submitting to PhilPapers
  • Frequently Asked Questions
  • Subscriptions
  • Editor's Guide
  • The Categorization Project
  • For Publishers
  • For Archive Admins
  • PhilPapers Surveys
  • Bargain Finder
  • About PhilPapers
  • Create an account

A dissertation on the origin of languages

Reprint years, call number, other versions.

No versions found

PhilArchive

External links.

  • This entry has no external links. Add one .

Through your library

  • Sign in / register and customize your OpenURL resolver
  • Configure custom resolver

Similar books and articles

Citations of this work.

No citations found.

The origin of language: A scientific approach to the study of man

  • Published: September 1985
  • Volume 4 , pages 181–186, ( 1985 )

Cite this article

dissertation language origin

  • Rüdiger Schreyer 1  

110 Accesses

2 Citations

Explore all metrics

The Enlightenment regarded language as one of the most significant achievements of man. Consequently inquiries into the origin and development of language play a central role in eighteenth-century moral philosophy. This new ‘science of man’ consciously adopts the method of analysis and synthesis used in the natural sciences of the time. In moral philosophy, analysis corresponds to the search for the basic principles of human nature. Synthesis is identified with the attempt to interpret all ‘artificial’ achievements of man (arts, sciences and institutions) as the effect of these principles and of man's physical and social environment - an attempt known as ‘theoretical history’. The type of explanation envisaged by theoretical historians is based on the principle of causality. It consists in a genetic reconstruction of the social phenomenon under investigation. Inquiries into the origin of language follow this pattern of explanation. They form part of theoretical history and thus represent a major aspect of the eighteenth-century scientific study of man.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save.

  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime

Price includes VAT (Russian Federation)

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Rent this article via DeepDyve

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

dissertation language origin

Theory of language: a taxonomy

dissertation language origin

Plato, Wittgenstein and the Origins of Language

dissertation language origin

Some Principles Relevant to the Evolution of Language

Aarsleff, Hans: 1974, ‘The tradition of Condillac: The problem of the origin of language in the Eighteenth century and the debate in the Berlin Academy before Herder’, Studies in the History of Linguistics. Traditions and Paradigms , ed. by Dell Hymes, Indiana U.P., Bloomington, pp. 93–156.

Google Scholar  

Aarsleff, Hans: 1982, ‘An outline of language-origins theories since the Renaissance’, From Locke to Saussure. Essays on the Study of Language and Intellectual History ’, Athlone, London, pp. 278–292.

Allen, Don Cameron: 1949, ‘Some theories of the growth and origin of language in Milton's age’, Philological Quarterly 28 , 5–16.

Allen, W. Sydney: 1948, ‘Ancient ideas on the origin and development of language’, Transactions of the Philological Society 81 , 35–60.

Article   Google Scholar  

Becker, Carl L.: 1932, The Heavenly City of the Eighteenth-Century Philosophers , Yale U.P., New Haven & London.

Borst, Arno: 1957–63, Der Turmbau von Babel. Meinungen über Ursprung und Vielfalt der Sprachen und Völker , 4 vols, in 6, Anton Hiersemann, Stuttgart.

Campbell, Robin N. and Robert Grieve: 1982, ‘Royal investigations of the origin of language’, Historiographia Linguistica 9 , 43–74.

Collingwood, Robin George: 1961 (1946), The Idea of History , Oxford U.P., Oxford.

Collingwood, Robin George: 1965 (1945), The Idea of Nature , Clarendon Press, Oxford.

De Gérando, Joseph-Marie: 1969 (1800), The Observation of Savage Peoples , transl, by F. C. T. Moore, University of California Press, Berkeley.

Ferguson, Adam: 1980 (1767), An Essay on the History of Civil Society , with a new introduction by Louis Schneider, Transaction Books, New Brunswick & London.

Formigari, Lia: 1974, ‘Language and society in the late Eighteenth Century’, Journal of the History of Ideas 35 , 275–92.

Hall, A. Rupert: 1962, The Scientific Revolution 1500–1800. The Formation of the Modern Scientific Attitude , Longman, London.

Herder, Johann Gottfried: n.d. (1772), Über den Ursprung der Sprache. Text, Materialien, Kommentar , ed. by Wolfgang Proβ, Hanser, München & Wien.

Hewes, Gordon Winant (ed.): 1975, Language Origins. A Bibliography , 2 vols., Mouton, The Hague & Paris.

Hobbes, Thomas: 1968 (1651), Leviathan, or the Matter, Forme & Power of a Common-Wealth Ecclesiasticall and Civill , ed. with an introduction by C. B. Macpherson, Penguin, Harmondsworth.

Hodgen, Margaret Trabue: 1964, Early Anthropology in the Sixteenth and Seventeenth Centuries , University of Philadelphia Press, Philadelphia.

Hume, David: 1955 (1748), An Inquiry Concerning Human Understanding , ed. and introduced by Charles W. Hendel, Bobbs-Merrill, Indianapolis & New York.

Hume, David: 1978 (1739), A Treatise of Human Nature: Being an Attempt to Introduce the Experimental Method of Reasoning into Moral Subjects , ed. with an analytical index by L. A. Selby-Bigge, 2nd edition with text revised and variant readings by P. H. Nidditch, Clarendon Press, Oxford.

Kuehner, Paul: 1944, Theories on the Origin and Formation of Language in the Eighteenth Century in France , University of Pennsylvania Press, Philadelphia.

Malthus, Thomas Robert: 1970 (1798), An Essay on the Principle of Population and a Summary view of the Principle of Population , ed. with an introduction by Anthony Flew, Penguin, Harmondsworth.

Megill, Allan Dickson: 1974, The Enlightenment Debate on the Origin of Language , Diss. Columbia University, New York.

Newton, Isaac: 1964 (1779–1785), Opera Quae Extant Omnia , Faksimile-Neudruck der Ausgabe von Samuel Horsley, in fünf Bänden, Friedrich Fromman, Stuttgart-Bad Cannstadt, Vol. IV (Opticks).

Plumb, John Harold: 1979, The Death of the Past , Macmillan, London.

Rendall, Jane: 1978, The Origins of the Scottish Enlightenment 1707–1776 , Macmillan, London.

Robertson, William: 1778 (1777), The History of America , 2 vols., 2nd ed., Strahan, Cadell and Balfour, London & Edinburgh.

Scharf, Joachim-Hermann: 1980, ‘Glottogenese and Sprachevolu-Tion’ Historiographia Linguistica 7 , 369–80.

Schreyer, Rüdiger: 1978, ‘Condillac, Mandeville and the origin of language’, Historiographia Linguistica 5 , 15–43.

Schreyer, Rüdiger: 1984, ‘Evidence and belief. Arguments in the eighteenth century debate on the origin of language’, Matériaux pour une histoire des théories linguistiques , ed. by Sylvain Auroux, Michel Glatigny, André Joly and Irène Rosier, 325–336. Université de Lille III, Lille.

Smith, Adam: 1980, Essays on Philosophical Subjects , ed. by W. P. D. Wightman et al. , Oxford U.P., Oxford.

Smith, Adam: 1970 (1767), A Dissertation on the Origin of Languages or Considerations Concerning the First Formation of Languages and the Different Genius of Original and Compounded Languages , ed. and introduced by Gunter Narr, Gunter Narr, Tübingen.

Stam, James Henry: 1976, Inquiries into the Origin of Language: The Fate of a Question , Harper and Row, New York & London.

Stewart, Dugald: 1980, ‘Account of the life and writings of Adam Smith’, in Smith, 1980.

Stewart, Dugald: 1877, The Collected Works of Dugald Stewart , 11 vols., ed. by Sir William Hamilton, T. and T. Clark, Edinburgh.

Teggart, Frederick J.: 1925, Theory of History , Yale U.P., New Haven.

Teggart, Frederick J.: 1949, The Idea of Progress. A Collection of Readings , revised edition with an introduction by George H. Hildebrand, University of California Press, Berkeley & Los Angeles.

Viertel, John: 1966, ‘Concepts of language underlying the 18th century controversy about the origin of language’, Monograph Series on Languages and Linguistics , ed. by Francis P. Dinneen, Georgetown University Press, Washington D.C.

Download references

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Rheinisch Westfälische Technische Hochschule, Aachen, The Netherlands

Rüdiger Schreyer

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Schreyer, R. The origin of language: A scientific approach to the study of man. Topoi 4 , 181–186 (1985). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00135846

Download citation

Issue Date : September 1985

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00135846

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Human Nature
  • Eighteenth Century
  • Scientific Method
  • Moral Philosophy
  • Intellectual History
  • Find a journal
  • Publish with us
  • Track your research
Format
BibTeX View Download
MARCXML View Download
TextMARC View Download
MARC View Download
DublinCore View Download
EndNote View Download
NLM View Download
RefWorks View Download
RIS View Download

Linked e-resources

Browse subjects.

  • Spelling reform.
  • English language Orthography and spelling.">Orthography and spelling.
  • English language Grammar">Grammar Grammar > Early works to 1800.">Early works to 1800.
  • Spelling reform Early works to 1800.">Early works to 1800.
  • Architecture and Design
  • Asian and Pacific Studies
  • Business and Economics
  • Classical and Ancient Near Eastern Studies
  • Computer Sciences
  • Cultural Studies
  • Engineering
  • General Interest
  • Geosciences
  • Industrial Chemistry
  • Islamic and Middle Eastern Studies
  • Jewish Studies
  • Library and Information Science, Book Studies
  • Life Sciences
  • Linguistics and Semiotics
  • Literary Studies
  • Materials Sciences
  • Mathematics
  • Social Sciences
  • Sports and Recreation
  • Theology and Religion
  • Publish your article
  • The role of authors
  • Promoting your article
  • Abstracting & indexing
  • Publishing Ethics
  • Why publish with De Gruyter
  • How to publish with De Gruyter
  • Our book series
  • Our subject areas
  • Your digital product at De Gruyter
  • Contribute to our reference works
  • Product information
  • Tools & resources
  • Product Information
  • Promotional Materials
  • Orders and Inquiries
  • FAQ for Library Suppliers and Book Sellers
  • Repository Policy
  • Free access policy
  • Open Access agreements
  • Database portals
  • For Authors
  • Customer service
  • People + Culture
  • Journal Management
  • How to join us
  • Working at De Gruyter
  • Mission & Vision
  • De Gruyter Foundation
  • De Gruyter Ebound
  • Our Responsibility
  • Partner publishers

dissertation language origin

Your purchase has been completed. Your documents are now available to view.

Pidginization and Creolization of Languages: Their Social Contexts

Sociolinguistic debates around the definitions and significance of “pidgin” and “creole” languages were increasing in the 1960s and the SSRC’s Committee on Sociolinguistics played a role in cultivating these discussions. This 1968 report by Dell Hymes summarizes issues raised at a conference convened by the Council at the University of the West Indies, Jamaica, to better understand the historical development, the grammatical and lexical evolutions, and the social uses of pidgin and creole languages. Though he highlights how social science can better inform research on pidginization and creolization, Hymes identifies knowledge gaps, among them the nature of the relationship between these languages and national identity, and more broadly the lack of historical and social scientific knowledge of this topic.

Languages called “pidgins” and “creoles” have been something of a stepchild in scientific research, but their origins and social functions pose in particularly clear form problems of the sort with which the Council’s Committee on Sociolinguistics is concerned. In analyzing historical change and in describing present structure in language, linguists often find it possible to take social factors for granted. Work proceeds as if something that might be called “normal transmission” of speech from one generation to the next could be assumed, or as if the sample of speech provided by one’s informants could be safely assumed to represent a norm identical throughout the community. Whether these assumptions are justified is open to question; what is clear is that even the ordinary work of the linguist cannot proceed without questioning them in the case of “pidgins” and “creoles.” These languages demonstrate dramatically the interdependence of linguistics and social science, and open up new possibilities for the integration of their methods and theories. During the past decade there has been a notable growth of interest and information concerning such languages, whose implications have not yet been widely recognized.

For these reasons an international conference was organized to encourage research on situations of pidginization and creolization, and call attention to its importance. The conference was cosponsored by the committee and the University of the West Indies, which has been the principal site of the development of creole studies in the past decade, and was held at the campus of the University in Mona, Jamaica, on April 9–12, 1968. By meeting in Jamaica, the conference was able to benefit from the participation of a number of Caribbean scholars for whom creolized languages are of personal and practical, as well as theoretical, importance. The depth and realism of some of the discussion reflected their presence.

Most of the participants in the conference were linguists, but many of them had some social science training and about one of four was affiliated with a social science department. [1] The papers prepared for the conference were grouped for discussion according to several main topics of concern: general conceptions of the nature of pidginization and creolization; analysis of specific characteristics and processes; reconstruction of the origins and history of such languages; recognition of the past occurrence of creolization in the history of a language; analysis of contemporary pidginization and creolization; and studies of the social role of pidgin and creole languages in contemporary communities. Two social scientists and two linguists were given the special task of reviewing the conference at its last session: Sidney Mintz (an anthropologist specializing in Caribbean cultures) from the standpoint of social history; Henry Hoenigswald from the standpoint of a specialist in comparative and historical linguistics; Allen Grimshaw as a member of the Council’s Committee on Comparative Sociological Research; and William Labov from the standpoint of an innovator in sociolinguistic analysis.

“Pidgin” and “creole”: Background

The conference sought to focus attention on processes, and to avoid popular connotations of the terms “pidgin” and “creole.” The terms have clear and standard scientific meanings. A pidgin is defined as a stable form of speech that is not learned as a first language (mother tongue) by any of its users, but as an auxiliary language by all; whose functions are sharply restricted (e.g., to trade, supervision of work, administration, communication with visitors), and whose vocabulary and overt structure are sharply reduced, in comparison with those of the languages from which they are derived. A creole is defined as an ordinary language that is derived from a pidgin and that through one or another set of circumstances has become the first language of a community, has been adapted to the full range of functions of community life, and has become notably richer in lexicon and structure than the pidgin from which it arose.

In most circumstances in which creoles are found they are considered socially inferior, even though sometimes thought superior in expressiveness. In ordinary usage the term pidgin suggests a mishmash, and certainly inferiority; only rarely, as among some peoples of New Guinea, is knowledge of a pidgin a badge of cosmopolitanism and male superiority. At best these languages are considered marginal; at worst, debased forms of speech without structure or value. Such attitudes have made them seem unworthy of study to most of their users and to those concerned with them officially, and have perhaps contributed to the relative absence of their study by others.

The common etymology for pidgin reflects these views. It is thought to be an Asian corruption of English “business,” although the changes in pronunciation are unparalleled and unlikely. In fact, the word is a sixteenth-century English “corruption” of a South American Indian term (Pidian) applied to the people with whom one traded and hence to the language used in trade (cf. Indian: Injun). The term and form of speech were carried over the world in the early years of European expansion and colonization. Indeed, one major theory is that almost all the world’s pidgins and creoles have their origin in an Afro-Portuguese pidgin developed on the coasts of West Africa, itself perhaps adapted from a Portuguese version of the medieval Mediterranean pidgin, Sabir, and subsequently rapidly replenished in vocabulary (“relexified”) from Spanish, English, Dutch, or French, as the case might be, in various parts of the Caribbean, the Pacific, and Asia.

Though restricted in content and use, pidgins are real languages with structures of their own that their users must learn. Pidgin English is not just any distortion of English that comes to mind, but a specific code, indeed a series of specific codes, not mutually intelligible with standard English or necessarily with each other, as between the various sectors of Asia, the Pacific, and Africa in which they are found. There is a fair amount of literature in some pidgins, through translation and local composition.

There are probably several million persons in the world who daily use some pidginized language. Languages characterized as creoles are spoken by more than six million persons in and around the Caribbean, by a variety of groups in West Africa (Sierra Leone, Camerouns, Ivory Coast, Guinea especially), and in Asia (India, Macao, the Philippines), as well as in South Africa and Indonesia, if the creole characteristics of Afrikaans and Bahasa Indonesia are taken into account. The major contemporary cases in the United States, recognized as such, are Gullah, once spoken widely in Georgia, South Carolina, and the Sea Islands, and Hawaiian pidgin and creole.

The circumstances that give rise to such forms of speech continue to occur: in Central Africa a pidginized Sango is spreading, while the ordinary Sango from which it derives (with the aid of French) may be dying out, according to William Samarin; in Katanga a pidginized form of Swahili is reported by Edgar Polomé to be undergoing creolization. The Pidgin English (Neo-Melanesian) of New Guinea is spreading as an integrative force among speakers of New Guinea’s many diverse languages, and may become the national language of the new country; an incipiently creolized Swahili has been adopted as the national language of administration and education in Tanzania.

Creoles, of whose status as full languages there is no question, are major factors in literature and education in the Caribbean and increasingly in England, and some contend that the perspective of the student of creole languages sheds light on the nature of some forms of English in use among Negroes in the United States (a point ably argued by Joseph Dillard). In short, education, administration, and sometimes quest for national identity are bound up with such languages in several parts of the world.

Simplification

In recent years linguists have tended to avoid questions of differences in complexity and adequacy among languages. With pidgins the questions are inherent in the subject. Much of the discussion at the first session of the conference revolved around the notions of “simplification” or “reduction.” Samarin treated pidginization as any “consistent reduction of the functions of language both in its grammar and its use.” So regarded, pidginization is part of a wide range of phenomena, including what the British sociologist Basil Bernstein has called “restricted codes”; much of the interest in the subject lies in this relationship. From this point of view, one undertakes a general study of simplifications and reductions of speech in adaptation to others. The social context is patently crucial, since simplification attributable to lack of shared understanding must be distinguished from simplification which represents the economy of means possible to those whose shared understandings are great. Keith Whinnom emphasized, in contrast to Samarin, the rarity with which simplification and mixture of speech have led to establishment of a pidgin. He compared cocoliche , a highly unstable and variable variety of speech found among Italian immigrants in Argentina, with Chinese Pidgin English. Cocoliche survived only as renewed by fresh immigrants from Italy, its potential second-generation speakers being instead speakers of Spanish. Whinnom made clear that the circumstances under which a pidgin can emerge must be quite specialized and stressed the process not only of simplification, but also of stabilization of a discrete form of speech not mutually intelligible with the languages from which it derives.

Whinnom suggested characteristics also stressed by William Stewart as essential to effective pidginization: a multilingual situation; separation from the domain of use of languages of wider communication; marginality of the speakers among whom the pidgin arises, such that they are not corrected by, or integrated among, the users of established linguistic norms. Apparently, there must also be sufficient difference among the languages involved, so that interference of one set of linguistic habits with imperfectly acquired others has a marked effect. (In an original comparison of biological and linguistic theory, Whinnom discussed primary and secondary hybridization in a revealing way, and specified the formation of pidgins as “tertiary” hybridization.)

This discussion raised most of the major issues of the conference: the characteristics distinctive of these languages; what the characteristics imply about users of the languages; the linguistic and social prerequisites of the processes involved; theories as to the origin of known pidgins and creoles.

Samarin reported on statistical studies of characteristics of pidgins and other forms of speech. Charles Ferguson noted that societies have varieties of speech that they themselves regard as simpler than others, and as suited for use with babies and foreigners. Their conventions must be studied as possibly shaping the outcome of pidginization. For example, absence of copula (forms of “to be” in English) is generally regarded, both by linguists and native speakers, as simpler than its presence. Ferguson proposed a series of relevant hypotheses. Their testing would help establish universal principles of simplicity as between forms of speech, and shed light on universals of language.

It was observed that the reduction of overt structure in pidgins may be accompanied by greater use of other modes of communication (intonation, gesture, facial expression); that greater cognitive effort may be involved in communicating with the restricted lexical and grammatical means of a pidgin; that there may be compensating complexities (as in length of sentences); and that the essential reduction of machinery special to particular languages could be seen as laying bare a substratum common to all.

John Reinecke, whose Yale dissertation thirty years ago was the first systematic American study of pidgin languages, described the Pidgin French spoken in Vietnam (Tay Boi), now vanishing with the withdrawal of the French. It is a classic case, with pronunciation essentially Vietnamese or French, according to the speaker, vocabulary from French, morphology simplified in the direction of Vietnamese, copula almost never used, and verbal means often eked out by gestures and intonation. Charles Frake analyzed the Zamboangueno dialect of Philippine Creole Spanish, whose history poses a number of problems for usual assumptions as to the nature and direction of lexical influence of one language on another. Of particular interest was the semantic structure associated with words of Philippine origin. Spanish vs. Philippine origin of words is not correlated with differences in the provenience of objects denoted (foreign vs. indigenous), but with the “unmarked” vs. “marked” in the sense of Joseph Greenberg’s general theory. Philippine-derived words are, in contrast with those of Spanish origin, markers of the smaller, nearer, younger, female, plural, or worse of a pair. Where forms are differentiated by style, the Spanish term marks formality (politeness toward addressee), and the Philippine is the unmarked conversational form. This indicated to the conference a quite unexpected and original possibility of linking the analysis of creolization to the study of language universals.

Common origin

A principal reason for the growth of attention to pidgins and creoles has been the hypothesis, developed only in the last decade, that most or all of them may have a common historical origin. The hypothesis is far from established, but it has already brought students of pidgin and creole languages together in a common field, where previously the study of one such language had been largely cut off from the study of others.

Some support for the hypothesis has appeared in a study by Morris Goodman that points to a common origin for all French-based creoles. Ian Hancock is advancing a similar thesis for English-based creoles. The general thesis is that there was an early Portuguese-based pidgin, rapidly stabilized, and readily relexified. From this standpoint, Chinese Pidgin English, for example, would not have arisen from confrontation of English and Chinese, but through adaptation of a pre-existing pidgin by speakers of Chinese. Frederic Cassidy discussed the linking of a pidgin element in Jamaican vocabulary to such a source. Such analysis of origins entails greater complexity of argument than can be summarized here, but clearly it is inseparable from social history, from specification of the location and movement of peoples at specific times, and from comparative analysis of the types of social situation in which communication occurred. Mervyn Alleyne challenges the Portuguese hypothesis, so far as Caribbean creoles are concerned, on just such grounds. He rejects, as do other creolists, the picture, derived in part from Leonard Bloomfield, of Europeans in each separate situation speaking baby talk or the like to inferiors, who in good faith adopt such talk while introducing features of their own language; and he accepts a common general origin for Caribbean creoles on the west coast of Africa; but he insists that the situation must be seen from the standpoint of Africans, learning one or another European language, and reinterpreting it in terms of patterns common to West African languages, leading to a syncretism in language like that well known in culture. Rather than a rapid crystallization of pidgins, Alleyne sees persistence of a continuum of variation from the most to the least standard (English, French), with the eventual outcome depending on the development of the total acculturation situation. Where the European linguistic model was withdrawn (as English was withdrawn from Surinam when Dutch was introduced), the creole end of the continuum is set off as a separate new language in a simple bilingual situation. Where the European model remains, as in Jamaica, the creole portion of the continuum moves steadily toward the standard, so that those who envisage an earlier dichotomy between the creole and standard language speak of the present situation as a postcreole continuum and predict the disappearance through absorption of the distinction.

Crosscutting this issue of the social process is the question of linguistic classification. Some would classify Haitian Creole as a dialect of French, Jamaican Creole as a dialect of English, etc., depending upon the European language from which the bulk of the vocabulary and, apparently, of morphological detail derives. Perhaps such classification is in part an effort to confer prestige upon the languages. Others point to the common grammatical patterns of Caribbean creoles whose lexical stocks are from diverse European languages, as evidence for their descent from a common pidgin ancestor, variously relexified, and as an indication that they are truly “new” languages, not properly to be classified or interpreted in terms of the usual methods of historical and comparative linguistics. In support of this view is the fact that basic vocabulary (the common core of meanings for body parts, natural objects, and the like found in all languages) tends to change at about the same rate in all languages, except pidgins (New Guinea Pidgin English) where the rate of change is wildly accelerated. From either point of view the different rates of change in basic vocabulary of pidgins and creoles challenge prevailing theory and recent procedures.

In a detailed study of Marathi and other Indo-Aryan languages of India, Franklin Southworth suggested that pidginization and creolization may have intervened in the history of indigenous Dravidian languages. John Gumperz provided evidence of almost total convergence in all but vocabulary forms between two contemporary Indian languages, one Dravidian (Kannada), one Indo-Aryan (a dialect of Marathi). Morris Goodman presented the problem of Mbugu, a language in Tanzania, whose grammatical structure pointed to Bantu origins, and whose basic vocabulary apparently did not. While inconclusive, these studies directed attention to the fact that if prior pidginization and creolization could have intervened widely in the past history of languages, many accepted classifications and subgroupings of languages would be of questionable validity. The great desideratum would be to establish linguistic, or social, criteria for the occurrence of the processes. Much more linguistic and social analysis is needed for this purpose. The results might considerably change usual approaches to linguistic prehistory and linguistic change.

Present social roles

Much of the interest of the conference, especially given the Caribbean setting, was in the status and consequences of creoles in contemporary societies. Jan Voorhoeve and Christian Eersel analyzed the situation in Surinam (former Dutch Guiana). A prestige variety of Sranan Tongo (“Church Creole”) was developed by missionaries, and Voorhoeve’s translation of the Bible into the nonprestigious Sranan vernacular is still under debate and cannot be used. Eersel analyzed choice between use of Dutch and Sranan Tongo in political affairs, personal relations between sexes and persons of different status, parents and children, etc. Sranan is becoming standardized and a vehicle of poetry. One comparison of interest is that to speak Dutch with standard grammar and vocabulary is good, but to speak it with a standard pronunciation is to put on airs. To speak standard Sranan with a standard Dutch (“bakara”) pronunciation, conversely, does confer prestige.

David DeCamp, Beryl Bailey, and Robert Le Page directed attention to problems of describing the actual state of affairs in a complex multilingual situation, wherein any given speaker (as in Jamaica) has command of not one norm, but a set of norms spanning part of a continuum. All introduced novel methodological devices for coping with such situations. DeCamp proposed a seven-point linguistic spectrum for Jamaica, and discussed ways of incorporating the multiplicity of varieties defined in the spectrum within a single set of rules of the sort used in transformational generative grammar. He argued that the limitation of a given speaker to some part of the continuum could find a place within a single grammar of the whole, and called for study of the factors that govern the actual switching of speakers within the span at their command—factors whose operation is as yet unknown. Bailey established creole and standard as two ideal types, introducing the number of rules required to go from both types to a given text as a measure for categorizing it as one or the other. Le Page argued for starting with the individual speaker as the basis from which regularities and relationships must be built up. Comments on the papers raised many questions which descriptive linguistics has only begun to answer.

Dennis Craig raised the question of the cognitive consequences of use of a creole language (a point that Whinnom had also discussed), and reported studies of the effects of teaching of standard English in schools. There was considerable discussion of the interplay between features of language, education, attitudes, and social structure in Caribbean communities and others, such as Hawaii, and of the future of the creole speakers in them.

In his concluding statement Sidney Mintz asked why present-day creoles are so largely concentrated in the Caribbean, and in the French and English (rather than Spanish) parts of it. He answered in terms of preconditions for creolization, giving a succinct demographic and historical analysis of a massively imported population caught up in quasi-urbanizing plantation life. Henry Hoenigswald spoke of the historical linguist as having two main interests in these languages: in their histories (genesis, change, disappearance), and in the relations of the phenomena of pidginization and creolization, and those of social change in general. In synthesizing many aspects of the discussion, he concentrated on the possible destructive consequences (noted above) of study of creoles and pidgins on notions of change, descent, family tree, and classification; and recommended clarification of the traditional concepts. Attention to classic cases in Indo-European showed the critical role of basic vocabulary in permitting a decision as to historical affiliation. There may be no sharp differentiation between pidginization and ordinary change in some sort of continuous space. Allen Grimshaw vigorously reviewed the development of comparative sociology, its relations with linguistics, and their areas of common interest. William Labov discussed types of linguists with respect to their methods and criteria for accepting results. Mentioning the near despair of some linguists in their efforts to write grammatical rules, he outlined several strategies for combining social and linguistic analysis to obtain convincing results.

Several memoranda indicating a variety of research needs had been circulated in advance of the conference. If a single result can be said to have emerged from the conference, it is somewhat ambiguous: retrospectively, considerable satisfaction in seeing the study of pidginization and creolization change from the marginal work of a few pioneers to a central object of research and theory; prospectively, a sense of urgency concerning the great amount of research needed to resolve the many problems brought into focus. There are still too few adequate descriptions of too few of the world’s pidgin and creole situations; too few linguists able to approach the historical origins of these languages with a knowledge of their putative African sources; too few linguists able to study the use and consequences of these languages in a manner informed by social science. Some of the Caribbean scholars urged particularly that the relation of these languages to questions of national identity and literature be given concentrated attention at some future conference. It is likely that the next decade will see as great a transformation of our knowledge in all these respects as has the last.

Items Editor’s note: The author is Professor of Anthropology, University of Pennsylvania. As a member of the Council’s Committee on Sociolinguistics, he was responsible, in collaboration with Gertrud Buscher, Lecturer in French at the University of the West Indies, for organizing the conference on which he reports here. The conference was made possible by a grant to the Council from the National Science Foundation, and funds allocated by the University of the West Indies.

Article note

This archival document has been reproduced the way it was initially published, with no further editing nor formatting of the bibliography.

© 2020 Hymes, published by De Gruyter

This work is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.

  • X / Twitter

Supplementary Materials

Please login or register with De Gruyter to order this product.

International Journal of the Sociology of Language

Journal and Issue

Articles in the same issue.

dissertation language origin

IMAGES

  1. (PDF) Origins of Language:

    dissertation language origin

  2. The Origin of Language, 978-3-659-87176-4, 3659871761 ,9783659871764 Di

    dissertation language origin

  3. The Origin of Language

    dissertation language origin

  4. (PDF) On the Origin of Language

    dissertation language origin

  5. An Etymological Dictionary of the Scottish Language : To Which Is

    dissertation language origin

  6. How to write a Dissertation

    dissertation language origin

VIDEO

  1. |The Origin Of English Language Is From Sanskrit|

  2. HOW TO DO PROJECT/DISSERTATION IN ENGLISH LITERATURE

  3. Origin of Language 1 in Urdu Hindi

  4. English editing, Proofreading and Medical Writing

  5. Michigan Language Assessment Spotlight: Tahnee Bucher, Ph.D

  6. How Did Language Begin? An Introduction to Linguistics

COMMENTS

  1. Language: Its Origin and Ongoing Evolution

    2.1. Modality of Origin. The debate on the modality of origin, which is the initial main channel carrying verbal information, also has a few contesting theories: according to the "gesture-first" view, "language evolved initially from manual gestures with vocal elements gradually added" (Corballis 2011, p. 383).The "speech-first" view (Dunbar 1997; MacNeilage 2008) argues for the ...

  2. Introduction: Origin and Evolution of Language—An ...

    5 Present Issue. This special issue provides an interdisciplinary view on contemporary language evolution research. It opens with two articles, those of Nathalie Gontier and Francesco Suman, which address epistemological issues concerning the relation between theory of evolution and language origin research.

  3. Introduction

    The origin of language has for centuries been the subject of fascination, and sometimes of ridicule. Countless hypotheses have been put forward about how, why, when and where language may have evolved. These range from the theory of "divine origin", to the more earthbound notion that language was a human invention, right up to the curious ...

  4. 1 The Origins and the Evolution of Language

    Speculations about the origins of language and linguistic diversity date from far back in the history of mankind. Among the most cited cases is the book of Genesis, in the Judeo-Christian Bible. After God created Adam, He reportedly gave him authority to name every being that was in the Garden of Eden. Putatively, God and Adam spoke some ...

  5. (PDF) On the Origin of Language

    On the Origin of Language. A Bridge Between Biolinguistics and Biosemiotics. Marcello Barbieri. Received: 14 January 2010 / Accepted: 23 April 2010 /. Published online: 23 May 2010. #. Springer ...

  6. The origin and diversification of language

    "Morris Swadesh, one of this century's foremost scientific investigators of language, dedicated much of his life to the study of the origin and evolution of language. This volume, left nearly completed at his death and edited posthumously by Joel F. Sherzer, is his last major study of this difficult subject.

  7. PDF THE ORIGIN OF LANGUAGE: AN ANTHROPOLOGICAL APPROACH

    which language evolved and within which it develops ontogenetically with each person. There is a large literature on language origins, acquisition, and capacity. It includes studies of linguistic processes that are impaired or elicited by point electrodes inserted into diverse language areas of the left hemisphere (Ojeman, 1983).

  8. PDF The origins of language

    The origins of language The suspicion does not appear improbable that the progenitors of man, either the males or females, or both sexes, before they had acquired the power of expressing their mutual love in articulate language, endeavoured to charm each other with musical notes and rhythm. Darwin (1871)

  9. (PDF) Linguistics: The Origin and Diversification of Language. MORRIS

    Swadesh begins with. a. review of the. intellectual history of two questions: the. origin of language, and the source (s) of the. many languages known to have existed. Many scholars consider ...

  10. New Essays on the Origin of Language

    The contributions to this volume reflect the state of the art in the renewed discussion on the origin of language. Some of the most important specialists in the field - life scientists and linguists - primarily examine two aspects of the question: the origin of the language faculty and the evolution of the first language. At stake is the relation between nature and culture and between ...

  11. Free Full-Text

    With the present paper, we sought to use research findings to illustrate the following thesis: the evolution of language follows the principles of human evolution. We argued that language does not exist for its own sake, it is one of a multitude of skills that developed to achieve a shared communicative goal, and all its features are reflective of this. Ongoing emerging language adaptations ...

  12. On the Origin of Languages

    This book presents a series of illuminating studies which conclusively demonstrates that the prevailing conception of historical linguistics is deeply flawed. Most linguists today believe that there is no good evidence that the Indo-European family of languages is related to any other language family, or even any other language. In like manner, the New World is deemed to contain hundreds of ...

  13. Origin of language

    The origin of language, its relationship with human evolution, and its consequences have been subjects of study for centuries.Scholars wishing to study the origins of language must draw inferences from evidence such as the fossil record, archaeological evidence, contemporary language diversity, studies of language acquisition, and comparisons between human language and systems of animal ...

  14. PDF A History of the English Language

    He is edi-tor of volume 1 of The Cambridge History of the English Language (six volumes, 1992-2001) and one of the founding editors of the journal English Language and Linguistics (also published by Cambridge University Press). He is author of Metrical Phonology with Christopher McCully (Cambridge University Press, 1986), A Grammar of Old ...

  15. Origins of Language

    The purpose of this chapter is to further review and discuss the historical origins of language, departing from the fundamental distinction between lexical evolution and grammatical evolution. Throughtout the paper, it will be emphasized that language represents the fundamental instrument of cognition, as was proposed by Vygotsky ( 1934/1962 ...

  16. PDF The Origin of Language

    Key Words: the origin of language, onomatopoeias, communication, birdsong, gestures, mutation brought them unto Adam to see what he would call them; and whatsoever Adam called every living crea-ture, that was the name thereof. And Adam gave names to all cattle, and to the fowl of the air, and to every beast of the field. (Genesis 2, 19-20) But ...

  17. The Origin of Language

    This book, The Origin of Language: Tracing the Evolution of the Mother Tongue, originally published in 1994 by John Wiley & Sons, was written in a more popular style, accessible to an educated general audience, than the more scholarly and academic tome of a similar title, On the Origin of Languages: Studies in Linguistic Taxonomy, published the same year. In The Origin of Language Ruhlen laid ...

  18. A dissertation on the origin of languages

    To Which is Added a Dissertation on the Origin of Languages. Adam Smith - 1767 - Printed for A. Millar, A. Kincaid and J. Bell in Edinburgh; and Sold by T. Cadell. The Theory of Moral Sentiments, or, an Essay Towards an Analysis of the Principles by Which Men Naturally Judge. To Which is Added, a Dissertation on the Origin of Languages.

  19. A dissertation on the origin of languages: or, considerations

    A dissertation on the origin of languages: or, considerations concerning the first formation of languages and the different genius of original and compounded languages. Adam Smith. Narr, 1970 - Language and languages - 104 pages. From inside the book . Common terms and phrases.

  20. The origin of language: A scientific approach to the study of man

    The Enlightenment regarded language as one of the most significant achievements of man. Consequently inquiries into the origin and development of language play a central role in eighteenth-century moral philosophy. This new 'science of man' consciously adopts the method of analysis and synthesis used in the natural sciences of the time. In moral philosophy, analysis corresponds to the ...

  21. Dissertations on the English language

    Dissertations on the English language [electronic resource] : with notes, historical and critical, to which is added, by way of appendix, an essay on a reformed mode of spelling, with Dr. Franklin's arguments on that subject. By Noah Webster, Jun. Esquire. [Two lines in Latin from Tacitus]. Webster, Noah, 1758-1843.; Franklin, Benjamin, 1706-1790.

  22. Pidginization and Creolization of Languages: Their Social Contexts

    Sociolinguistic debates around the definitions and significance of "pidgin" and "creole" languages were increasing in the 1960s and the SSRC's Committee on Sociolinguistics played a role in cultivating these discussions. This 1968 report by Dell Hymes summarizes issues raised at a conference convened by the Council at the University of the West Indies, Jamaica, to better understand ...