OPINION article

Factors affecting impulse buying behavior of consumers.

\nRosa Isabel Rodrigues

  • Instituto Superior de Gestão, Lisbon, Portugal

In recent years, the study of consumer behavior has been marked by significant changes, mainly in decision-making process and consequently in the influences of purchase intention ( Stankevich, 2017 ).

The markets are different and characterized by an increased competition, as well a constant innovation in products and services available and a greater number of companies in the same market. In this scenario it is essential to know the consumer well ( Varadarajan, 2020 ). It is through the analysis of the factors that have a direct impact on consumer behavior that it is possible to innovate and meet their expectations. This research is essential for marketers to be able to improve their campaigns and reach the target audience more effectively ( Ding et al., 2020 ).

Consumer behavior refers to the activities directly involved in obtaining products /services, so it includes the decision-making processes that precede and succeed these actions. Thus, it appears that the advertising message can cause a certain psychological influence that motivates individuals to desire and, consequently, buy a certain product/service ( Wertenbroch et al., 2020 ).

Studies developed by Meena (2018) show that from a young age one begins to have a preference for one product/service over another, as we are confronted with various commercial stimuli that shape our choices. The sales promotion has become one of the most powerful tools to change the perception of buyers and has a significant impact on their purchase decision ( Khan et al., 2019 ). Advertising has a great capacity to influence and persuade, and even the most innocuous, can cause changes in behavior that affect the consumer's purchase intention. Falebita et al. (2020) consider this influence predominantly positive, as shown by about 84.0% of the total number of articles reviewed in the study developed by these authors.

Kumar et al. (2020) add that psychological factors have a strong implication in the purchase decision, as we easily find people who, after having purchased a product/ service, wonder about the reason why they did it. It is essential to understand the mental triggers behind the purchase decision process, which is why consumer psychology is related to marketing strategies ( Ding et al., 2020 ). It is not uncommon for the two areas to use the same models to explain consumer behavior and the reasons that trigger impulse purchases. Consumers are attracted by advertising and the messages it conveys, which is reflected in their behavior and purchase intentions ( Varadarajan, 2020 ).

Impulse buying has been studied from several perspectives, namely: (i) rational processes; (ii) emotional resources; (iii) the cognitive currents arising from the theory of social judgment; (iv) persuasive communication; (v) and the effects of advertising on consumer behavior ( Malter et al., 2020 ).

The causes of impulsive behavior are triggered by an irresistible force to buy and an inability to evaluate its consequences. Despite being aware of the negative effects of buying, there is an enormous desire to immediately satisfy your most pressing needs ( Meena, 2018 ).

The importance of impulse buying in consumer behavior has been studied since the 1940's, since it represents between 40.0 and 80.0% of all purchases. This type of purchase obeys non-rational reasons that are characterized by the sudden appearance and the (in) satisfaction between the act of buying and the results obtained ( Reisch and Zhao, 2017 ). Aragoncillo and Orús (2018) also refer that a considerable percentage of sales comes from purchases that are not planned and do not correspond to the intended products before entering the store.

According to Burton et al. (2018) , impulse purchases occur when there is a sudden and strong emotional desire, which arises from a reactive behavior that is characterized by low cognitive control. This tendency to buy spontaneously and without reflection can be explained by the immediate gratification it provides to the buyer ( Pradhan et al., 2018 ).

Impulsive shopping in addition to having an emotional content can be triggered by several factors, including: the store environment, life satisfaction, self-esteem, and the emotional state of the consumer at that time ( Gogoi and Shillong, 2020 ). We believe that impulse purchases can be stimulated by an unexpected need, by a visual stimulus, a promotional campaign and/or by the decrease of the cognitive capacity to evaluate the advantages and disadvantages of that purchase.

The buying experience increasingly depends on the interaction between the person and the point of sale environment, but it is not just the atmosphere that stimulates the impulsive behavior of the consumer. The sensory and psychological factors associated with the type of products, the knowledge about them and brand loyalty, often end up overlapping the importance attributed to the physical environment ( Platania et al., 2016 ).

The impulse buying causes an emotional lack of control generated by the conflict between the immediate reward and the negative consequences that the purchase can originate, which can trigger compulsive behaviors that can become chronic and pathological ( Pandya and Pandya, 2020 ).

Sohn and Ko (2021) , argue that although all impulse purchases can be considered as unplanned, not all unplanned purchases can be considered impulsive. Unplanned purchases can occur, simply because the consumer needs to purchase a product, but for whatever reason has not been placed on the shopping list in advance. This suggests that unplanned purchases are not necessarily accompanied by the urgent desire that generally characterizes impulse purchases.

The impulse purchases arise from sensory experiences (e.g., store atmosphere, product layout), so purchases made in physical stores tend to be more impulsive than purchases made online. This type of shopping results from the stimulation of the five senses and the internet does not have this capacity, so that online shopping can be less encouraging of impulse purchases than shopping in physical stores ( Moreira et al., 2017 ).

Researches developed by Aragoncillo and Orús (2018) reveal that 40.0% of consumers spend more money than planned, in physical stores compared to 25.0% in online purchases. This situation can be explained by the fact that consumers must wait for the product to be delivered when they buy online and this time interval may make impulse purchases unfeasible.

Following the logic of Platania et al. (2017) we consider that impulse buying takes socially accepted behavior to the extreme, which makes it difficult to distinguish between normal consumption and pathological consumption. As such, we believe that compulsive buying behavior does not depend only on a single variable, but rather on a combination of sociodemographic, emotional, sensory, genetic, psychological, social, and cultural factors. Personality traits also have an important role in impulse buying. Impulsive buyers have low levels of self-esteem, high levels of anxiety, depression and negative mood and a strong tendency to develop obsessive-compulsive disorders. However, it appears that the degree of uncertainty derived from the pandemic that hit the world and the consequent economic crisis, seems to have changed people's behavior toward a more planned and informed consumption ( Sheth, 2020 ).

Author Contributions

All authors listed have made a substantial, direct and intellectual contribution to the work, and approved it for publication.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Aragoncillo, L., and Orús, C. (2018). Impulse buying behaviour: na online-offline comparative and the impact of social media. Spanish J. Market. 22, 42–62. doi: 10.1108/SJME-03-2018-007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burton, J., Gollins, J., McNeely, L., and Walls, D. (2018). Revisting the relationship between Ad frequency and purchase intentions. J. Advertising Res. 59, 27–39. doi: 10.2501/JAR-2018-031

Ding, Y., DeSarbo, W., Hanssens, D., Jedidi, K., Lynch, J., and Lehmann, D. (2020). The past, present, and future of measurements and methods in marketing analysis. Market. Lett. 31, 175–186. doi: 10.1007/s11002-020-09527-7

Falebita, O., Ogunlusi, C., and Adetunji, A. (2020). A review of advertising management and its impact on consumer behaviour. Int. J. Agri. Innov. Technol. Global. 1, 354–374. doi: 10.1504/IJAITG.2020.111885

Gogoi, B., and Shillong, I. (2020). Do impulsive buying influence compulsive buying? Acad. Market. Stud. J. 24, 1–15.

Google Scholar

Khan, M., Tanveer, A., and Zubair, S. (2019). Impact of sales promotion on consumer buying behavior: a case of modern trade, Pakistan. Govern. Manag. Rev. 4, 38–53. Available online at: https://ssrn.com/abstract=3441058

Kumar, A., Chaudhuri, S., Bhardwaj, A., and Mishra, P. (2020). Impulse buying and post-purchase regret: a study of shopping behavior for the purchase of grocery products. Int. J. Manag. 11, 614–624. Available online at: https://ssrn.com/abstract=3786039

Malter, M., Holbrook, M., Kahn, B., Parker, J., and Lehmann, D. (2020). The past, present, and future of consumer research. Market. Lett. 31, 137–149. doi: 10.1007/s11002-020-09526-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meena, S. (2018). Consumer psychology and marketing. Int. J. Res. Analyt. Rev. 5, 218–222.

Moreira, A., Fortes, N., and Santiago, R. (2017). Influence of sensory stimuli on brand experience, brand equity and purchase intention. J. Bus. Econ. Manag. 18, 68–83. doi: 10.3846/16111699.2016.1252793

Pandya, P., and Pandya, K. (2020). An empirical study of compulsive buying behaviour of consumers. Alochana Chakra J. 9, 4102–4114.

Platania, M., Platania, S., and Santisi, G. (2016). Entertainment marketing, experiential consumption and consumer behavior: the determinant of choice of wine in the store. Wine Econ. Policy 5, 87–95. doi: 10.1016/j.wep.2016.10.001

Platania, S., Castellano, S., Santisi, G., and Di Nuovo, S. (2017). Correlati di personalità della tendenza allo shopping compulsivo. Giornale Italiano di Psicologia 64, 137–158.

Pradhan, D., Israel, D., and Jena, A. (2018). Materialism and compulsive buying behaviour: the role of consumer credit card use and impulse buying. Asia Pacific J. Market. Logist. 30,1355–5855. doi: 10.1108/APJML-08-2017-0164

Reisch, L., and Zhao, M. (2017). Behavioural economics, consumer behaviour and consumer policy: state of the art. Behav. Public Policy 1, 190–206. doi: 10.1017/bpp.2017.1

Sheth, J. (2020). Impact of Covid-19 on consumer behavior: will the old habits return or die? J. Bus. Res. 117, 280–283. doi: 10.1016/j.jbusres.2020.05.059

Sohn, Y., and Ko, M. (2021). The impact of planned vs. unplanned purchases on subsequent purchase decision making in sequential buying situations. J. Retail. Consumer Servic. 59, 1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.jretconser.2020.102419

Stankevich, A. (2017). Explaining the consumer decision-making process: critical literature review. J. Int. Bus. Res. Market. 2, 7–14. doi: 10.18775/jibrm.1849-8558.2015.26.3001

Varadarajan, R. (2020). Customer information resources advantage, marketing strategy and business performance: a market resources based view. Indus. Market. Manag. 89, 89–97. doi: 10.1016/j.indmarman.2020.03.003

Wertenbroch, K., Schrift, R., Alba, J., Barasch, A., Bhattacharjee, A., Giesler, M., et al. (2020). Autonomy in consumer choice. Market. Lett. 31, 429–439. doi: 10.1007/s11002-020-09521-z

Keywords: consumer behavior, purchase intention, impulse purchase, emotional influences, marketing strategies

Citation: Rodrigues RI, Lopes P and Varela M (2021) Factors Affecting Impulse Buying Behavior of Consumers. Front. Psychol. 12:697080. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.697080

Received: 19 April 2021; Accepted: 10 May 2021; Published: 02 June 2021.

Reviewed by:

Copyright © 2021 Rodrigues, Lopes and Varela. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY) . The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Rosa Isabel Rodrigues, rosa.rodrigues@isg.pt

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Impulse buying: a meta-analytic review

  • Review Paper
  • Open access
  • Published: 09 July 2019
  • Volume 48 , pages 384–404, ( 2020 )

Cite this article

You have full access to this open access article

  • Gopalkrishnan R. Iyer 1 ,
  • Markus Blut 2 ,
  • Sarah Hong Xiao 3 &
  • Dhruv Grewal 4  

87k Accesses

181 Citations

92 Altmetric

10 Mentions

Explore all metrics

Impulse buying by consumers has received considerable attention in consumer research. The phenomenon is interesting because it is not only prompted by a variety of internal psychological factors but also influenced by external, market-related stimuli. The meta-analysis reported in this article integrates findings from 231 samples and more than 75,000 consumers to extend understanding of the relationship between impulse buying and its determinants, associated with several internal and external factors. Traits (e.g., sensation-seeking, impulse buying tendency), motives (e.g., utilitarian, hedonic), consumer resources (e.g., time, money), and marketing stimuli emerge as key triggers of impulse buying. Consumers’ self-control and mood states mediate and explain the affective and cognitive psychological processes associated with impulse buying. By establishing these pathways and processes, this study helps clarify factors contributing to impulse buying and the role of factors in resisting such impulses. It also explains the inconsistent findings in prior research by highlighting the context-dependency of various determinants. Specifically, the results of a moderator analysis indicate that the impacts of many determinants depend on the consumption context (e.g., product’s identity expression, price level in the industry).

Similar content being viewed by others

research papers on buying

Compulsive Buying Behavior: Relationship with Impulse Buying and a Proposed Model of Antecedents

research papers on buying

Online Impulse Buying – Integrative Review of Social Factors and Marketing Stimuli

research papers on buying

Investigating Impulse Buying and Variety Seeking: Towards a General Theory of Hedonic Purchase Behaviors

Avoid common mistakes on your manuscript.

Consumers spend $5,400 per year on average on impulse purchases of food, clothing, household items, and shoes (O’Brien 2018 ). Thus, there is considerable need to investigate consumer impulse buying, defined as episodes in which “a consumer experiences a sudden, often powerful and persistent urge to buy something immediately” (Rook 1987 , p. 191). Products purchased impulsively often get assigned to a distinct category in marketing texts, yet decades of research reveal that impulsive purchases actually are not restricted to any specific product category. As Rook and Hoch ( 1985 , p. 23) assert, “it is the individuals, not the products, who experience the impulse to consume.”

Academic research that explores the various triggers of impulse buying consists of three main schools of thought. First, some scholars argue that individual traits lead consumers to engage in impulse buying (e.g., Verplanken and Herabadi 2001 ). For example, people who are impulsive are more likely to engage in impulse buying (Rook and Hoch 1985 ), whereas those who do not display this trait may be less likely to engage in spontaneous behaviors while shopping. Among the psychological factors that might evoke impulse buying, researchers have explored the traits of sensation seeking, impulsivity, and representations of self-identity. Second, both motives and resources might drive impulse buying. Researchers have identified the effects of two types of motives (hedonic and utilitarian), as well as subjective norms, and argued that mere impulsiveness is often not strong enough to trigger impulse buying. Instead, the availability of resources coupled with a failure of self-control also is required to enact impulse buying (Baumeister 2002 ; Hoch and Loewenstein 1991 ). Considerable research has investigated the specific influences of different types of resources, including psychic, time, and money resources (Vohs and Faber 2007 ), with the assumption that resource-based motives, availability, and constraints impact consumer impulse buying. Third, some studies focus on the role of marketing drivers, highlighting how impulse buying can result from store or shelf placements, attractive displays, and in-store promotions. This view holds that impulse buying can be influenced, so retailers invest in marketing instruments designed to trigger it (Mattila and Wirtz 2001 ).

Although these diverse research streams approach impulse buying from different angles and have established considerable insights into its triggers, a unified and comprehensive view of the drivers of impulse buying would further enhance our understanding. We perform a meta-analysis on an accumulation of prior empirical research, focusing on disparate drivers and the most impactful antecedents, and the substantive insights obtained from the estimation of effect sizes. Our study can guide further research and the results also could aid managers in crafting strategies to stimulate impulse purchases by targeting the most receptive customers and investing in effective marketing campaigns. In addition to the direct effects of various antecedents on impulse buying, our proposed framework identifies several mediating mechanisms, including self-control (Vohs and Faber 2007 ) and positive and negative emotions (Rook and Gardner 1993 ). We test the joint effects of emotions and self-control, which enables us to specify their concurrent mediating roles, as well as the potential for serial moderation (i.e., self-control influences emotions). Apart from the typical study moderators, we examine industry moderators—namely, the average price level, advertising, and distribution intensity in the industry, as well as the identity expression capacity of the product category—in line with Rook and Fisher’s ( 1995 , p. 312) call for “a better understanding of various contextual factors that are also likely to contribute to this relationship [between determinants and impulse buying].” The precise roles of these moderating variables have not been explored in prior impulse buying studies, and a better understanding of their influence can provide new insights and spur further in-depth research.

Our use of a meta-analysis is in line with calls in recent research (Grewal et al. 2018b ; Palmatier et al. 2018 ) highlighting the importance of such integrative reviews. An earlier meta-analysis by Amos et al. ( 2014 ) summarized the impacts of various factors on consumer impulse buying; our review extends on their work in several ways. First, we recognize the diverse perspectives on impulse buying and the need to obtain a more comprehensive understanding by combining insights from different research streams. To this end, we have sourced extensively and include 186 papers in our meta-analysis, compared with 63 in Amos et al. ( 2014 ). Second, Amos et al. focus primarily on main effects, whereas we examine moderators and mediators, in addition to the main effects. This scrutiny of the moderating effects also allows us to consider individual relationships rather than pool the effect sizes of all antecedents (Amos et al. 2014 ) and thus identify stronger and weaker effects. Third, by examining mediating effects, we can test alternate theory-based relationships of the various antecedents on impulse buying. The resulting insights help provide a more inclusive understanding of impulse buying as compared with the use of only one theoretical perspective.

Conceptual framework

Several determinants of impulse buying appear in prior research. In line with Dholakia ( 2000 ), we explore the effects of trait determinants, motives, resources, and marketing stimuli on impulse buying. Beyond these categories of main effects, our integrated model explores their impacts through the mediation of self-control and individual emotional states as well (Mehrabian and Russell 1974 ). We also account for contextual differences in effects by examining the moderating influences of industry-related characteristics. Furthermore, we consider the possible influence of study characteristics (i.e., impulse buying measure, sample composition, and publication year) on the effects obtained. Our conceptual model is in Fig.  1 , and we offer a summary of the predicted relationships in Table  1 .

figure 1

Meta-analytic framework

Determinants of impulse buying

Trait and related determinants.

Several individual traits and self-identity may serve as internal sources of impulse buying. Psychological impulses strongly influence impulse buying (Rook 1987 ; Rook and Hoch 1985 ), and prior research shows that people who score high on impulsivity trait measures are more likely to engage in impulse buying (Beatty and Ferrell 1998 ; Rook and Fisher 1995 ; Rook and Gardner 1993 ). Moreover, other traits are also associated with impulse buying and studies in the past have attempted to study their impacts as well (e.g., Mowen and Spears 1999 ; Sharma et al. 2010 ).

First, we examine the role of sensation-seeking as having a direct impact on impulse buying. Sensation-seeking, variety-seeking, novelty-seeking, and similar dispositions are arguably distinct from other traits such as impulsivity and reported as contributing to impulse buying (Punj 2011 ; Sharma et al. 2014 ; Van Trijp and Steenkamp 1992 ). Second, an impulse buying tendency , which includes the trait of impulsivity, reflects an enduring disposition to act spontaneously in a specific consumption context. This well-recognized concept captures a relatively enduring consumer trait that produces an urge or motivation for actual impulse buying (Rook and Fisher 1995 ). Impulse buying tendencies, are easier to observe than other traits and are also highly predictive of impulse buying (Beatty and Ferrell 1998 ; Rook and Gardner 1993 ). Third, buyer-specific beliefs about self-identity and its deficits influence impulse buying decisions (Dittmar et al. 1995 ). Impulse purchases are more likely to involve items that are symbolic of a preferred or ideal self as well as products that offer high identity-expressive potential, to compensate for the buyer’s own identity deficits (Dittmar et al. 1995 ; Dittmar and Bond 2010 ). However, contextual factors may play a role on the impacts of such perceptions of identity deficits (e.g., Dittmar et al. 2009 ).

Motives and norms

Consumers’ motives, such as hedonic or utilitarian motives , are important internal sources of impulse buying that reflect goal-directed arousal, leading to specific beliefs about consumption. For example, consumers may believe that buying objects will provide emotional gratification, compensation, rewards, or else minimize their negative feelings. Such beliefs may be especially relevant if the objects are unique and feature a marked opportunity cost, such that they need to be purchased immediately (Rook and Fisher 1995 ; Vohs and Faber 2007 ).

Norms invoked by consumers about their own impulsiveness also might affect impulse buying decisions. As Rook and Fisher ( 1995 , p. 307) explain, “consumers’ own prior impulse buying experiences may serve as a basis for independent, internalized evaluations of impulse buying as either bad or good.” From a self-regulation perspective, when prior impulse buying evokes positive experiences, consumers likely engage in it again, as a promotion-focused strategy (Verplanken and Sato 2011 ).

Customers with greater psychic resources or interest in a product category are more likely to engage in impulse buying, whereas those who lack the necessary resources (time, money) engage less in impulse buying (Hoch and Loewenstein 1991 ; Jones et al. 2003 ; Kacen and Lee 2002 ). Age and gender might capture shopping-related resources, such that impulse buying tendencies often are more prevalent among specific social or demographic cohorts (Kacen and Lee 2002 ; Tifferet and Herstein 2012 ; Wood 1998 ). Drawing from prior research, Kacen and Lee ( 2002 ) offer that younger shoppers may be more likely to buying impulsively while older adults may be better able to regulate their emotions and engage in self-control.

Several research and practical observations have highlighted gender differences in shopping (e.g., Underhill 2000 ). Dittmar et al. ( 1995 ) find that men and women are likely to buy different products to buy impulsively and also use different buying considerations when buying on impulse. Also, it has been found that women are more likely as compared to men to experience regret or a mixture of pleasure and guilt (Coley and Burgess 2003 ).

  • Marketing stimuli

Marketers deliberately design external stimuli to appeal to shoppers’ senses (Eroglu et al. 2003 ). Managers expend substantial time and effort in designing retail environments and the resulting retail interactions to increase shoppers’ psychological motivation to purchase (Berry et al. 2002 ; Foxall and Greenley 1999 ). It has been estimated that about 62% of in-store purchases are made impulsively and online buyers are more likely to be impulsive (Chamorro-Premuzic 2015 ). Thus, impulse buying can be triggered by various marketing stimuli such as merchandise, communications, store atmospherics, and price discounts (Mohan et al. 2013 ).

Mediators of impulse buying

Baumeister ( 2002 ) has established the importance of motives and resource depletion for driving impulse buying; therefore, we also consider whether self-control and emotions might be triggered. By including these mediating mechanisms in our meta-analysis, we avoid over- or underestimating the importance of various impulse buying triggers. In particular, we assess the joint effects of emotions and self-control, which enables us to specify their concurrent mediating roles, as well as the potential for serial mediation (i.e., self-control influences emotions).

Self-control as a mediator

Countering prior arguments that impulse purchases stem from irresistible urges, Baumeister ( 2002 ) has argued that individuals’ self-control can and do resist such urges. Muraven and Baumeister ( 2000 ; p. 247) submit that self-control, or the “control over the self by the self,” involves attempts by individuals to curb their desires, conform to rules and change how think, feel or act. Also, individuals differ in self-control leading to the view that self-control is an inherent strength or trait (Baumeister 2002 ). It has also been argued that a failure of self-control could occur due to conflicting goals, reduction in self-monitoring or depletion of mental resources (Baumeister 2002 ; Verplanken and Sato 2011 ). The depletion of mental resources, or “ego depletion,” may also be temporal, i.e., more likely to occur at the end of the day (Baumeister 2002 ; p. 673). The “ever-shifting conflict between desire and willpower” (Vohs and Faber 2007 , p. 538) demonstrates the importance of self-control as a key mediator in the impacts of various antecedents noted in our model and impulse buying.

Emotions as mediators

Environmental psychology research, and particularly the stimulus–organism–response model proposed by Mehrabian and Russell ( 1974 ), highlights experienced emotions as potential mediating constructs. Input variables such as environmental stimuli or individual traits jointly influence individual affective responses, which then induce response behaviors (Baker et al. 1992 ). Verplanken and Herabadi ( 2001 ) explain that customers engaging in impulse buying tend to display emotions at any point of time during the purchase (i.e., before, during, or after). Extant findings are somewhat inconsistent though. It has been argued that impulse buying behavior relates strongly to positive emotions and feelings such that impulse buyers experience more positive emotions such as delight and consequently spend more (Beatty and Ferrell 1998 ). Impulse buyers have a strong need for arousal and experience an emotional lift from persistent repetitive purchasing behaviors (O'Guinn and Faber 1989 ; Verplanken and Sato 2011 ). Such arousal even might be a stronger motive for impulse buying than product ownership (Dawson et al. 1990 ).

Rook and Gardner ( 1993 ) acknowledge that while pleasure is an important precursor, negative mood states such as sadness, can also be associated with impulse buying. For example, various studies suggest self-gifting to be a form of retail therapy that helps customers in managing their moods (Mick and Demoss 1990 ; Rook and Gardner 1993 ; Vohs and Faber 2007 ). Other researchers concur that impulse buying can serve to manage or elevate negative mood states but also suggest that this influence occurs through a self-regulatory function (Rook and Gardner 1993 ; Verplanken et al. 2005 ). Thus, emotional states—whether positive or negative—likely affect impulse buying, but we find no consensus about whether or how negative moods, positive moods, or both determine impulse buying uniquely.

Finally, research rooted in environmental psychology asserts that exposure to environmental stimuli, consumers’ personalities, and personal motives can cause specific (positive or negative) emotional reactions (e.g., Babin et al. 1994 ; Donovan and Rossiter 1982 ; Mehrabian and Russell 1974 ). These in turn mediate the impacts of personal, situational, and external factors on impulse buying (Parboteeah et al. 2009 ; Verhagen and van Dolen 2011 ). The limited empirical evidence on the mediating role of emotions refers to specific contexts; for example, Adelaar et al. ( 2003 ) show that pleasure, dominance, and arousal triggered at the moment of purchase mediate the effect of a media format on impulse buying intentions online. Verhagen and van Dolen ( 2011 ) found that positive emotions mediate the effects of consumer beliefs about online stores and their likelihood of buying impulsively. Store environments and circumstances such as time and money resources also might prompt negative emotional reactions (Lucas and Koff 2014 ; Vohs and Faber 2007 ), suggesting the need for more empirical evidence to determine which emotions are more prominent.

The serial mediation of self-control and emotions also deserves examination. The motivational role of self-control also suggests that a successful exercise of self-control may also contribute to positive affect; in other words, individuals with higher self-control not only resist temptations successfully but may experience other consequent states such as fewer emotional problems and greater life satisfaction (Baumeister 2002 ; Baumeister et al. 2008 ; Hofmann et al. 2012 ; Tice et al. 2001 ). The conceptualization of self-control as a strength and self-control failure as ego-depletion (c.f., Baumeister 2002 ) also paves the way for understanding how the exercise of self-control and the unpleasant consequence of self-regulation of a pleasant task may contribute to seeking other pleasurable pursuits (Finley and Schemichel 2018 ). Thus, individuals may counter the distasteful after-effects of a self-control act by pursuing opportunities that would contribute to positive emotions (Finley and Schemichel 2018 ). This view of self-control views ego-depletion as a process, whereby the exercise of self-control in one time period leads to the individual seeking subsequent positive experiences (Finley and Schemichel 2018 ). Another view of self-control offers that self-control may not be all about inhibitions and restrictions; the trait of self-control may also engage in a promotion focus and thereby engage in initiatory behaviors towards achieving the same goal (Cheung et al. 2014 ). While the above discussion sheds light on the relationship between self-control and positive emotions, there is a lack of clarity in current literature on the precise direction of the relationship between self-control and emotional states relative to impulse buying as well as the impact of self-control on negative emotions.

Contextual moderators

We seek novel insights by examining industry characteristics as potential contextual moderators. Based on extant studies, we identify the price levels, advertising, and distribution intensity within the industry context as moderators that may influence the effects of other factors on impulse buying. The identity expression capability of the products themselves could moderate the impacts of the various determinants too. Prior impulse buying studies do not test the effects of these moderators; to derive our predictions, we thus turn to relationship marketing research that reveals how industry-level variables determine effectiveness (Fang et al. 2008 ). Product price levels matter, because financial constraints suppress impulse purchases (Rook and Fisher 1995 ), and impulse buying triggers are less effective in more expensive product categories. In their meta-analysis, Samaha et al. ( 2014 ) find that advertising intensity in a specific industry reduces the effectiveness of a firm’s communication activities. We posit that similarly, impulse buying triggers may be less effective in industries in which all firms invest heavily in advertising, because consumers are less likely to recognize and consider these various triggers. In addition, distribution intensity in an industry might influence impulse buying, because the urge to purchase likely increases when products are rare or exclusive (Troisi et al. 2006 ). Finally, some products are more prone to impulse purchases, especially if they symbolize a preferred or ideal self (Dittmar et al. 1995 ; Dittmar and Bond 2010 ). Thus, we anticipate differing effectiveness of impulse buying triggers according to the product.

Method moderators

Meta-analyses frequently consider the influence of the methods adopted by the included studies, such as how they measure key constructs, on the strength of the focal relationships. Impulse buying studies frequently use different measures for similar constructs; we use the scale for buying impulse developed by Rook ( 1987 ) as a baseline to assess whether other measures perform differently. Meta-analyses also can reveal whether the use of specific samples influences the findings (Orsingher et al. 2009 ). In particular, student samples tend to be more homogeneous than non-student samples and thus produce stronger effect sizes. Finally, we assess the influence of the study period. The emergence of the Internet and advanced communication technologies have left customers more knowledgeable, with altered expectations of retailers (Blut et al. 2018 ). Accordingly, we consider whether customers’ impulse buying behaviors might have changed over time.

Data collection and coding

We collected the data for this study by searching electronic databases, including EBSCO, Proquest, Ingenta Journals, Elsevier Science Direct, Google Scholar, the web, and several pertinent leading journals (e.g., Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, Journal of Consumer Research, Journal of Marketing, Journal of Marketing Research ). We also identified relevant articles by examining the reference lists of the collected articles. Our search used various terms, including “impulse buying” and “impulsive buying,” “impulsivity,” “compulsive buying,” and “unplanned buying,” and encompassed titles, abstracts, and keywords. The document types included articles and reviews (c.f., book review); the language was English; and the subject areas spanned marketing and advertising, management, business, economics, sociology, and psychology. We also obtained some unpublished studies from their authors. We sent 159 emails to authors of published papers seeking at least minimally relevant statistics for conducting the analysis. After excluding theoretical papers, qualitative studies, book reviews, studies that mention but do not measure impulse buying, and studies that do not report the necessary effect sizes, we pared down the list of 386 articles to a final data set of 186 articles reporting empirical results. Footnote 1

We coded each effect size according to the relationship of the independent variables (traits, motives, resources, and marketing stimuli), the mediators (self-control, positive emotions, and negative emotions) and impulse buying. We also coded the industry and method moderator variables, such that we assessed industry characteristics (i.e., product-identity relation, price level, advertising intensity, and distribution intensity) using the industry description reported by the studies. We similarly coded the method moderators (i.e., study year, measurement of impulse buying, and student sample) using information provided in each study. Two coders achieved agreement greater than 90% and discussed any inconsistencies, using the construct definitions in Table  2 to classify all the variables.

We included studies that reported (1) correlations (r) between the variables of interest, (2) the standardized regression coefficients (beta coefficients), (3) F- or t-values, or (4) frequencies, to calculate as as many effect sizes, so as to enhance the generalizability (Peterson and Brown 2005 ).

Integration of effect sizes

Correlation coefficients were used as effect sizes in our meta-analysis. If such coefficients were not reported in the collected studies, we transformed alternative statistics, such as regression coefficients, into correlations (Peterson and Brown 2005 ). Following Peterson and Brown ( 2005 ), we imputed correlations from the beta coefficients using the formula: r = .98β + .05λ with λ = 1 when β > 0 and λ = 0 when β < 0. Some studies also report more than one correlation for the same relationship between two constructs, in which case, we averaged the two correlations and treated them as if they were from a single study (Hunter and Schmidt 2004 ). We did not have enough effect sizes to include some determinants in all analyses, such as the four marketing stimuli of communication, price stimuli, store ambience, and merchandise. We therefore examined these determinants separately when possible and merged them as necessary to include them in other analyses. If a study had measured more than one of the four instruments, we calculated an average effect size for the aggregate marketing stimuli variable. This approach ensures the use of only one aggregate marketing stimuli effect size for each study. After transforming and averaging the effect sizes, the total data set in the meta-analysis consists of 968 effect sizes, extracted from 231 samples obtained from 186 articles. The total combined sample includes 75,434 respondents.

We used a random-effects approach (Hunter and Schmidt 2004 ) to calculate the average correlations. Effect sizes were corrected for measurement error in the dependent and independent variables using the coded reliability coefficients. We followed the Hunter and Schmidt ( 2004 ) recommendation of dividing the correlations by the product of the square root of the respective reliabilities of the two constructs involved. Further, reliability-adjusted correlations were weighted by sample size to adjust for sampling error. It has been recommended that reliability-adjusted effect sizes should be transformed into Fisher’s z coefficients before weighting them by sample size (Kirca et al. 2005 ). This transformation is not without controversies, and some studies suggest that Fisher’s z overestimates true effect sizes by 15%–45% (Field 2001 ). However, when we compare the results of both approaches, we find no significant differences.

Next, for each sample size–weighted and reliability-adjusted correlation, we calculated standard errors and 95% confidence intervals. We used a chi-square test and applied a 75% rule-of-thumb to assess the homogeneity of the effect size distribution (Hunter and Schmidt 2004 ). To assess the robustness of our results and potential publication bias, we estimated Rosenthal’s ( 1979 ) fail-safe N; in other words, the estimation of the number of studies that had null results and therefore not published before the Type I error probability can be brought to a barely significant level ( p  = .05). We also tested the influence of sample size and effect size outliers on integrated effect sizes, but the results remained largely the same (Geyskens et al. 2009 ). To assess the practical relevance of the different determinants, we calculated the shared variance with impulse buying for each predictor, as well as the binomial effect size display (BESD) (Grewal et al. 2018b ), which indicates the likelihood that a customer (e.g., female) would purchase impulsively compared with a reference group (e.g., male customers). A value greater than 1 indicates a greater relative likelihood, whereas a value lower than 1 signals a lower likelihood.

Descriptive statistics

Direct effects.

As Table  3 indicates, the averaged effect sizes for most motives, resources, and trait predictors are significant; however, socio-demographic predictors seem to matter less for impulse buying. We find strong support for the impacts of the three trait-related predictors on impulse buying. As expected, an individual's tendency to act impulsively has a stronger effect than other traits, reflecting its stronger link to the behavior of interest.

Utilitarian and hedonic motives show about equal impacts on impulse buying; further research should pay more attention to these determinants. We find support for gender effects but observe no differences for age. The former results are in line with prior research that suggests women generally are more likely to purchase impulsively than men (Dittmar et al. 1995 ). However, the insignificant results for age suggests there are not many differences between older and younger customers with regard to spending money impulsively. Moreover, we find that marketing stimuli exert a direct influence on customers’ impulse buying behavior. When examining the specific marketing instruments, we find the strongest effects for communication and price stimuli and weaker effects for store ambience and merchandise.

We uncover significant effects for emotions and self-control (Table  4 ). Descriptive statistics were also examined to gauge the impact of the predictors on the mediators (Table 4 ); 30 of the 39 predictor–mediator relationships (77%) are significant. Thus, we obtain a preliminary indication of the mediating roles of emotions and self-control, and we can proceed to test the proposed mediating effects in the SEM.

The shared variances and BESD give some indication of the practical relevance of different determinants. Using these criteria, we observe strong effects of impulse buying tendencies, utilitarian motives, and communication. All the significant relationships are robust to publication bias because the file-drawer N is many times greater than the tolerance levels proposed by Rosenthal ( 1979 ). We also examined funnel plots and do not find any indication of publication bias. In all cases, the significant chi-square tests of homogeneity suggest moderation.

Evaluation of structural equation model

We tested the mediating effects using structural equation modeling (SEM) and included variables for which correlations with all other variables could be identified. The complete correlation matrix includes correlations between the most often studied variables in prior research (Table  5 ). It served as the input to LISREL 8.80 and the harmonic mean of all sample sizes ( N  = 1726) was used as input. Since the harmonic mean is lower than the arithmetic mean, SEM estimations are more conservative (Viswesvaran and Ones 1995 ). Note that since each construct had only a single indicator and since measurement errors were taken into account when estimating the mean effect sizes, the error variances in the SEM could be set to 0. The different marketing instruments could not be individually included in the SEM, due to the small number of effect sizes, so we aggregated all marketing instruments into one determinant variable and examined its influence in the SEM; if a study included two or more marketing stimuli effects, we averaged them. The proposed model with both mediators and the effect of self-control on emotions performs well and displays a good fit (Fig.  2 ).

figure 2

Results of the structural equation model. Notes : A dotted line indicates that the path is not significant. Model fit: χ 2 /1 = 67.74; confirmatory fit index = .99; goodness-of-fit index = .99; root mean residual = .02; standardized root mean residual = .02

Positive moods

The SEM results suggest that positive moods are important mediators (Fig. 2 ). Customers with stronger hedonic motives are more likely to experience positive feelings; customers with utilitarian motives are less likely to experience such feelings. Those with favorable subjective norms and high self-control also experience positive moods. These effects are new to extant impulse buying literature. Similarly, customers who are generally high in impulsivity experience positive feelings. Finally, marketing stimuli relate significantly to positive feelings, though the effect is relatively weak.

Negative moods

Negative mood states relate significantly to impulse buying, and each of the determinants link to this mediator, with the exception of marketing stimuli and self-control. Customers high in hedonic and utilitarian motives are less likely to experience negative moods. Favorable subjective norms increase the likelihood of negative feelings. Impulse buying tendency is positively related to the experience of negative moods. The insignificance of marketing stimuli suggests that the stimuli do not trigger negative moods in customers. Self-control also does not reduce the experience of negative emotions.

  • Self-control

Unlike mood states, self-control reduces the likelihood of impulse purchases. This cognition intervenes when customers experience an urge to buy impulsively. According to the SEM results, several predictors either trigger individual awareness of the long-term consequences of spending or reassure consumers that spending is acceptable. For example, customers high in impulsivity are less likely to exhibit self-control. Subjective norms that encourage impulse buying lower self-control perceptions, but marketing stimuli serve to increase self-control. Finally, hedonic and utilitarian motives increase self-control perceptions. The positive effect of marketing stimuli on self-control suggests that customers are aware of how firms try to influence them to make them impulsive purchases.

Similar to Pick and Eisend ( 2014 ), we tested the importance of mediation effects using two approaches. First, we examined the ratio of indirect effects to total effects as displayed in Table  6 . We find significant indirect effects and high ratios for most determinants, including self-control (20%), impulse buying tendency (46%), utilitarian motives (34%), norms (49%), and marketing stimuli (39%). Only the indirect effect of hedonic motives is insignificant, leading to a low ratio of indirect effects to total effects (8%). The direct, indirect, and total effects differ for some determinants; self-control has a negative direct effect on impulse buying, yet the indirect effect through mediators is positive, which mitigates the total negative effect. Impulse buying tendency has positive direct and indirect effects on impulse buying, such that the total effect is nearly twice as strong as the direct effect. Utilitarian motives have a positive direct effect on impulse buying and a negative indirect effect that lowers the total effect. Norms display a negative direct effect and a positive indirect effect; we observe the opposite effects for marketing stimuli. The mediation model thus provides a clearer view of how these determinants influence impulse buying.

Second, we compare the proposed model, which assumes partial mediation effects, with two models with only indirect effects of the determinants through moods and self-control (full mediation). As suggested by Pick and Eisend ( 2014 ), we compare the models using a chi-square difference test (Δχ 2 /df). Both full mediation models exhibit significantly worse model fit than the proposed model (mood: Δχ 2 /df = 630.51/6, p  < .01; self-control: Δχ 2 /df = 755.28/8, p  < .01). Thus, the mediating effects of moods and self-control are partial rather than full.

Moderator analysis results

The need for a moderator analysis was assessed through the chi-square test of homogeneity and a 75% rule (Hunter and Schmidt 2004 ). The 75% rule indicates that if the proportion of variance in the distribution of effect sizes attributed to sampling error and other artifacts is less than 75%, a moderator analysis is warranted. In our results, the chi-square value is significant in all cases, and the 75% rule suggests values lower than 75%, in support of a moderator analysis. We coded several moderators in our random effects regression model as dummy variables, including the four industry moderators: product identity relation (1 = high expressive, 0 = low expressive), price level (1 = high, 0 = low), advertising intensity (1 = high, 0 = low), and distribution intensity (1 = high, 0 = low). Footnote 2 For the two method moderators, impulse buying measure (1 = Rook, 0 = non-Rook) and sample (1 = student, 0 = non-student), we used dummy codes. The year of the study came directly from the articles.

Using meta-regression procedures suggested by Lipsey and Wilson ( 2001 ) and the provided macros, we assess the influence of the moderators in our model with random-effects regression (Hunter and Schmidt 2004 ). Using reliability-corrected correlations as the dependent variable, we conducted tests of the moderators for 18 predictor variables and regressed correlations on four industry variables and three method variables. To test moderation effects, we ensured that at least 10 effect sizes were available (Samaha et al. 2014 ).

Product identification

We confirm a moderating influence of product identification (Table  7 ). If a product’s expressiveness is high (i.e., product identity is coded as 1 for high expressiveness), some predictors lose their relevance, including self-identity and subjective norms. Products that facilitate consumer self-expression are more likely to be bought impulsively, because they represent a preferred or ideal self (Dittmar et al. 1995 ; Dittmar and Bond 2010 ). Products with high expressiveness also suppress the effects of norms. In these conditions, other determinants become less effective. However, some determinants related to communication and negative feelings gain importance, because consumers are very sensitive with regard to their self-perceptions.

Price level

As expected, the average price level of products in an industry buffers the impacts of several predictors. Most predictors lose some relevance when prices are high (i.e., price level is coded as 1), including sensation-seeking, impulse buying tendency, hedonic motives, utilitarian motives, psychic resources, and positive moods. Only self-control gains importance, in line with our reasoning. Higher prices alert consumers to the financial consequences of their urge to buy impulsively, making these determinants less effective (but self-control more effective).

Advertising intensity

The influence of advertising is quite interesting. On the one hand, it appears to increase desire for certain products, so some predictors gain relevance. On the other hand, the predictors may lose relevance, because products seem less unique when they are advertised everywhere. Negative moods and merchandise gain importance with greater advertising intensity, but norms, psychic resources, and store ambience matter less.

Distribution intensity

Product availability in an industry depends on its distribution intensity. For example, Dholakia ( 2000 ) explains that physical proximity is essential for the experience of an impulsive urge, but a product that is unusually difficult to purchase may be more appealing to customers than products that are available everywhere. We anticipated and find that at least some impulse buying predictors, such as utilitarian motives, psychic resources, merchandise, and negative mood states, become less effective when a product is more widely available. Moreover, communication gains relevance with greater distribution intensity.

When examining the moderating influence of the method adopted in the different studies, we find that several predictors, such as impulse buying tendency and utilitarian motives, gain importance over time. We do not observe a specific pattern for the measures employed. The results with regard to the measures used in the studies suggest that the widely employed Rook scale performs as well as alternative impulse buying measures. We also find generally weaker effects in studies using student samples. In further meta-regression models, we assessed the influence of country culture and emerging markets but do not find notable differences.

Implications and directions for further research

This meta-analysis aims to provide a comprehensive and coherent understanding of impulse buying behavior, by synthesizing previous research. Our meta-analytic review seeks deeper insights into impulse buying, and our comprehensive model of impulse buying integrates constructs and relationships from studies over the past four decades of empirical research on impulse buying. The results from our meta-analysis provide new insights into the impacts of various antecedent factors and call particular attention to the tensions between the inherent urge to buy impulsively and the constraints and control on such buying impulses. Also, the results clarify the impacts of marketing stimuli on consumer impulse buying and highlight the context-dependency of impulse buying research. These meta-analysis results in turn suggest several implications for practice and directions for further research.

Managerial implications

Consumer buying on impulse has long been an area of interest for managers; even a small proportion of impulse purchases on each shopping trip or a small base of impulse shoppers can contribute significant annual incremental sales (Rostoks 2003 ). It is therefore important to identify not just which consumers may be more inclined to purchase on impulse but also specific environmental factors that may prompt and encourage impulse buying. Impulse purchases can increase retail sales (top-line) and profits (bottom-line), especially for high-margin products. As summarized in Table  8 , our results suggest employing a variety of marketing strategies.

In their attempt to devise strategies to encourage impulse shopping and/or promote impulse buying behaviors, retailers have not been averse to making large investments in marketing stimuli, such as merchandising, displays, lighting, music, and other environmental factors that might trigger impulse purchases (Mattila and Wirtz 2001 ). Our review acknowledges that impulse buying can be triggered by external factors, so retailers should devise new, unique marketing stimuli to convey the value of their offerings and encourage impulse buying. Yet not all marketing stimuli are equally effective. Communication and price stimuli are more effective in prompting impulse buying than are store ambience and merchandise. Although retailers often devote considerable expenses to store design, store atmosphere, store layout, and merchandise placement, they may be better off investing more in price promotions and advertising, which likely have stronger impulse buying effects.

An important practical insight from this meta-analysis is that though marketing mix stimuli have positive impacts on impulse buying, they also heighten awareness of such tendencies and thus may curb impulse buying overall. This finding suggests consumers are becoming increasingly familiar with firms’ tactics to persuade them to buy impulsively and skeptical of various marketing practices. For practitioners, these findings may be somewhat discouraging; impulse buying is not simply a response to marketing stimuli, and psychological, social, and situational variables also have impacts. Additional research is warranted to understand how shopper skepticism evoked by marketing tactics might inhibit impulse buying. Retailers may need to try harder to devise unique or new marketing stimuli that can get past consumers’ defenses and convey the value of their offers.

The identification of an impulse buying segment of customers would be of great importance to retailers that currently rely solely on marketing stimuli. But if impulse buying were only trait driven, marketing strategy would have no effect on impulse purchases. The good news from our meta-analysis is that impulse buying is triggered by both factors internal to consumers and external marketing stimuli. Thus, it may be possible to identify consumers prone to impulse buying but also specify situations that enable it. That is, marketers could identify a distinct impulse buying segment and then design the shopping environment to make their impulse buying more likely. In some challenging findings though, we show that demographics such as age and gender matter less for predicting impulse buying, so retailers likely need to undertake deeper research into consumer psychographics to identify an impulse buying segment.

Shopping motives, whether hedonic or utilitarian, also matter when it comes to impulse buying. These motives are inherent to the consumer, so marketers should design stores and offers to evoke and facilitate appropriate motives. Yet consumers’ resource constraints (e.g., time, money) curb their buying impulses, so marketers also could focus on devising tactics to reduce the impacts of resource constraints. For example, access to speedy financing and faster checkouts likely help mitigate credit and time constraints.

Consumers with high self-control and those influenced by social norms also may be less prone to impulse buying, because the uninhibited urge to buy impulsively is curbed by self-control and social norms. Understanding these restrictions can help ethical marketers develop stimuli that both facilitate unplanned purchases but discourage purely uninhibited, impulsive purchases that may lead to later regret and consumer dissatisfaction. Ultimately, marketers must choose between making an immediate sale that might produce consumer dissatisfaction and exhibiting concern for the consumer to encourage future patronage. Similarly, both positive and negative emotions enhance impulse buying, and ethical marketers should leverage affective strategies to encourage impulsive purchases that align with available consumer resources. Public policy makers also might take heed of self-control, norms, and emotions to devise policies to reduce unhealthy impulse buying.

Because industry characteristics also matter in impulse buying, managers need to understand how the industry context moderates the impacts of various consumer traits, motives, and resources on impulse buying. Even if impulse buying is common in industries with low price levels, our findings caution that it is not the only relevant industry context; rather, impulse buying also occurs when product–identity relationships are strong. In such contexts, marketers should place due emphasis on communications that encourage impulse buying.

Directions for research

Our meta-analysis, while revealing, was restricted given the lack of sufficient studies testing and/or reporting all possible effects in all possible contexts using multiple methods. In exploring the main effects of various factors on impulse buying (Fig. 1 ), we had to use aggregations in several cases, due to the insufficient number of effects available in prior research. Future studies should undertake explicit examinations of each effect, especially specific marketing stimuli, self-identity, positive and negative moods, specific types of social norms, and consumer resources. The most glaring deficiencies in prior research provide the bases for our recommendations for further research, which we detail in Table  9 and summarize briefly here.

We indicate the effects of various individual drivers, including marketing stimuli, on impulse buying in Table 3 , which suggests an important facilitating role for impulse buying. We test the individual impacts of traits, motives, resources, and stimuli on impulse buying, but interactions among these antecedents also could be influential. For example, experimental research might determine how the effects of traits, motives, and resources on impulse buying are moderated by marketing stimuli (e.g., communication, price, store ambience, merchandise elements). The size of the motive effects (r = .34 for hedonic, r = .36 for utilitarian) implies their potential significance; they could be activated by communications delivered to customers in stores, using digital displays (Roggeveen et al. 2016 ) or mobile devices (Grewal et al. 2018a ). Furthermore, the synergistic effects of various communication and promotional elements on impulse buying warrant further exploration.

Most studies make assumptions about the context, rather than actively manipulating or exploring its effects. In most cases, the context refers solely to the product category (e.g., food, beauty products), shopping environment (e.g., retail store, online), or industry (grocery, apparel). But various other contextual cues could be relevant, such as consumer decision stage, whether consumption is private or public, demographic variables, and whether the shopper is alone or accompanied by someone (Table 9 ). Such contextual cues should function as moderators in future studies to help reveal how various antecedent factors affect impulse buying.

Studies exploring impulse buying also tend to use surveys and examine correlational data. Such descriptive analyses provide generalizable insights, though manipulations of various marketing stimuli, motives, and resources in experiments also could enable causal inferences. Longitudinal research that relies on panel data could also reveal how consumer motives and resources interact with the context to prompt impulse buying. New technologies, such as eye-tracking methods, could demonstrate the specific impacts of marketing stimuli (e.g., product placements) and how consumers’ attention paid to various details in the shopping environment contributes to their impulse buying. Finally, we find some evidence that is contradictory with theoretical predictions, so qualitative research would be helpful to explain why.

Our meta-analytic review aims to provide empirically generalizable, robust findings pertaining to the impacts of various antecedents of impulse buying, its potential mediators, and the moderators of these relationships. As a unique feature, our meta-analysis includes a test of alternate theoretical perspectives that previously have sought to explain impulse buying. As Palmatier et al. ( 2007 ) attest, on the basis of their comparative consideration of multiple theoretical perspectives on interorganizational relationships, various perspectives could receive empirical support individually, but their relative impacts cannot be determined unless all explanatory perspectives are subjected to a comparative test. With the greater number of effects sizes available for each model, achieved by compiling data for the meta-analysis, our comparative test of various perspectives on impulse buying brings the relative impacts of various dominant explanatory factors in each perspective into sharper relief.

In summary, our meta-analysis explores the direct effects of consumer traits, motives, and resources and marketing stimuli on impulse buying, along with the mediating impacts of self-control and positive and negative emotions. Our joint examination of these mediators reveals the inner affective and cognitive psychological processes of impulse buying and their relations. Industry and method moderators also influence impulse buying. This meta-analysis provides a comprehensive summary of extant research, underlying various implications. We hope it also sheds some new lights on directions for research that can continue to enhance our understanding of impulse buying.

The complete list of studies used in this meta-analysis is available from the authors.

For example, grocery retailing involves low product identity relation, low price level, high advertising intensity, and high distribution intensity; the luxury car industry was coded as high product identity relation, high price level, low advertising intensity, and low distribution intensity.

Abratt, R., & Goodey, S. D. (1990). Unplanned buying and in-store stimuli in supermarkets. Managerial and Decision Economics, 11 (2), 111–121.

Article   Google Scholar  

Adelaar, T., Chang, S., Lancendorfer, K. M., Lee, B., & Morimoto, M. (2003). Effects of media formats on emotions and impulse buying intent. Journal of Information Technology, 18 (4), 247–266.

Amos, C., Holmes, G. R., & Keneson, W. C. (2014). A meta-analysis of consumer impulse buying. Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services, 21 (2), 86–97.

Andrews, J. C., Durvasula, S., & Akhter, S. H. (1990). A framework for conceptualizing and measuring the involvement construct in advertising research. Journal of Advertising, 19 (4), 27–40.

Babin, B. J., Darden, W. R., & Griffin, M. (1994). Work and/or fun: Measuring hedonic and utilitarian shopping value. Journal of Consumer Research, 20 (4), 644–656.

Baker, J., Levy, M. L., & Grewal, D. (1992). An experimental approach to make retail store environmental decisions. Journal of Retailing, 68 (4), 445–460.

Google Scholar  

Baumeister, R. F. (2002). Yielding to temptation: Self-control failure, impulsive purchasing, and consumer behavior. Journal of Consumer Research, 28 (4), 670–676.

Baumeister, R. F., Sparks, E. A., Stillman, T. F., & Vohs, K. D. (2008). Free will in consumer behavior: Self-control, Ego depletion, and choice. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 18 (1), 4–13.

Beatty, S., & Ferrell, M. E. (1998). Impulse buying: Modeling its precursors. Journal of Retailing, 74 (2), 169–191.

Berry, L. L., Seiders, K., & Grewal, D. (2002). Understanding service convenience. Journal of Marketing, 66 (3), 1–17.

Billieux, J., Gay, P., Rochat, L., & Van der Linden, M. (2010). The role of urgency and its underlying psychological mechanisms in problematic behaviours. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48 (11), 1085–1096.

Blut, M., Teller, C., & Floh, A. (2018). Testing retail marketing-mix effects on patronage: A meta-analysis. Journal of Retailing, 94 (2), 113–135.

Chamorro-Premuzic, T. (2015), The Psychology of Impulsive Shopping, The Guardian , November 26, https://www.theguardian.com/media-network/2015/nov/26/psychology-impulsive-shopping-christmas-black-friday-sales [Accessed March 13, 2019].

Cheung, T. L., Gillebaart, M., Kroese, F., & Ridder, D. D. (2014). Why are people with higher self-control happier? The effect of trait self-control on happiness as mediated by regulatory focus. Frontiers in Psychology, 5 (July). https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00722 .

Cohen, J. B., & Andrade, E. B. (2004). Affective intuition and task-contingent affect regulation. Journal of Consumer Research, 31 (2), 358–367.

Coley, A., & Burgess, B. (2003). Gender differences in cognitive and affective impulse buying. Journal of Fashion Marketing and Management, 7 (3), 282–295.

Davis, R., & Sajtos, L. (2009). Anytime, anywhere: Measuring the ubiquitous Consumer's impulse purchase behavior. International Journal of Mobile Marketing, 4 (1), 15–22.

Dawson, S., Bloch, P. H., & Ridgway, N. M. (1990). Shopping motives, emotional states, and retail outcomes. Journal of Retailing, 66 (winter), 408–427.

Dholakia, U. M. (2000). Temptation and resistance: An integrated model of consumption impulse formation and enactment. Psychology & Marketing, 17 (11), 955–982.

Dittmar, H., & Bond, R. (2010). I want it and I want it Now': Using a temporal discounting paradigm to examine predictors of consumer impulsivity. British Journal of Psychology, 101 (4), 751–776.

Dittmar, H., Beattie, J., & Friese, S. (1995). Gender identity and material symbols: Objects and decision considerations in impulse purchases. Journal of Economic Psychology, 16 (3), 491–511.

Dittmar, H., Halliwell, E., & Stirling, E. (2009). Understanding the impact of thin media models on Women’s body-focused affect: The roles of thin-ideal internalization and weight-related self-discrepancy activation in experimental exposure effects. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 28 (1), 43–75.

Donovan, R. J., & Rossiter, J. R. (1982). Store atmosphere: An environmental psychology approach. Journal of Retailing, 58 (1), 34–57.

Eroglu, S. A., Machleit, K. A., & Davis, L. A. (2003). Empirical testing of a model of online store atmospherics and shopper responses. Psychology & Marketing, 20 (2), 139–150.

Fang, E., Palmatier, R. W., & Steenkamp, J. E. M. (2008). Effect of service transition strategies on firm value. Journal of Marketing, 72 (5), 1–14.

Field, A. P. (2001). Meta-analysis of correlation coefficients: A Monte Carlo comparison of fixed- and random-effects models. Psychological Methods, 6 , 161–180.

Finley, A. J., & Schemichel, B. J. (2018). Aftereffects of self-control on positive emotional reactivity. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 45 , 1011–1027. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167218802836 .

Fishbach, A., & Labroo, A. A. (2007). Be better or be merry: How mood affects self-control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93 (2), 158–173.

Foxall, G. R. (2007). Explaining Consumer Choice . Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.

Book   Google Scholar  

Foxall, G. R., & Greenley, G. E. (1999). Consumers' emotional responses to service environments. Journal of Business Research, 46 (2), 149–158.

Geyskens, I., Krishnan, R., Steenkamp, J. E. M., & Cunha, P. V. (2009). A review and evaluation of meta-analysis practices in management research. Journal of Management, 35 (2), 393–419.

Grewal, D., & Marmorstein, H. (1994). Market price variation, perceived price variation, and consumers' price search decisions for durable goods. Journal of Consumer Research, 21 (3), 453–460.

Grewal, D., Ahlbom, C., Beitelspacher, L. S., Noble, S. M., & Nordfalt, J. (2018a). In-store Mobile phone use and customer shopping behavior: Evidence from the field. Journal of Marketing., 82 (4), 102–126.

Grewal, D., Puccinelli, N., & Monroe, K. B. (2018b). Meta-analysis: Integrating accumulating knowledge. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 46 (1), 9–30.

Herabadi, A. G., Verplanken, B., & Van Knippenberg, A. (2009). Consumption experience of impulse buying in Indonesia: Emotional arousal and hedonistic considerations. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 12 (1), 20–31.

Hirschman, E. C., & Holbrook, M. B. (1982). Hedonic consumption: Emerging concepts, methods and propositions. Journal of Marketing, 46 (Summer), 92–101.

Hoch, S. J., & Loewenstein, G. F. (1991). Time-inconsistent preferences and consumer self-control. Journal of Consumer Research, 17 (4), 429–507.

Hofmann, W., Baumeister, R. F., Förster, G., & Vohs, K. D. (2012). Everyday temptations: An experience sampling study of desire, conflict, and self-control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102 (6), 1318–1325.

Hunter, J. E., & Schmidt, F. L. (2004). Methods of meta-analysis: Correcting error and Bias in research findings . Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications.

Jones, M. A., Reynolds, K. E., Weun, S., & Beatty, S. E. (2003). The product-specific nature of impulse buying tendency. Journal of Business Research, 56 (7), 505–511.

Kacen, J. J., & Lee, J. A. (2002). The influence of culture on consumer impulsive buying behavior. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 12 (2), 163–176.

Kirca, A. H., Jayachandran, S., & Bearden, W. O. (2005). Market orientation: A meta-analytic review and assessment of its antecedents and impact on performance. Journal of Marketing, 69 (2), 24–41.

Kukar-Kinney, M., Ridgway, N. M., & Monroe, K. B. (2012). The role of Price in the behavior and purchase decisions of compulsive buyers. Journal of Retailing, 88 (1), 63–71.

Kwon, H. H., & Armstrong, K. L. (2002). Factors influencing impulse buying of sport team licensed merchandise. Sport Marketing Quarterly, 11 (3), 151–163.

Lin, Y. H., & Chen, C. F. (2013). Passengers' shopping motivations and commercial activities at airports–the moderating effects of time pressure and impulse buying tendency. Tourism Management, 36 , 426–434.

Lipsey, M. W., & Wilson, D. B. (2001). Practical meta-analysis . Thousand Oaks: Sage.

Liu, Y., Li, H., & Hu, F. (2013). Website attributes in urging online impulse purchase: An empirical investigation on consumer perceptions. Decision Support Systems, 55 (3), 829–837.

Lucas, M., & Koff, E. (2014). The role of impulsivity and of self-perceived attractiveness in impulse buying in women. Personality Individual Differences, 56 (January), 111–115.

Luo, X. (2005). How does shopping with others influence impulsive purchasing? Journal of Consumer Psychology, 15 (4), 288–294.

Mattila, A. S., & Wirtz, J. (2001). Congruency of scent and music as a driver of in-store evaluations and behavior. Journal of Retailing, 77 (2), 272–289.

Mehrabian, A., & Russell, J. A. (1974). An approach to environmental psychology . Cambridge: MIT Press.

Mick, D. G., & Demoss, M. (1990). Self-gifts: Phenomenological insights from four contexts. Journal of Consumer Research, 17 (3), 322–332.

Mohan, G., Sivakumaran, B., & Sharma, P. (2013). Impact of store environment on impulse buying behavior. European Journal of Marketing, 47 (10), 1711–1732.

Morrin, M., & Chebat, J. C. (2005). Person-place congruency: The interactive effects of shopper style and atmospherics on consumer expenditures. Journal of Service Research, 8 (2), 181–191.

Mowen, J. C., & Spears, N. (1999). Understanding compulsive buying among college students: A hierarchical approach. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 8 (4), 407–430.

Muraven, M., & Baumeister, R. F. (2000). Self-regulation and depletion of limited resources: Does self-control resemble a muscle?. Psychological Bulletin, 126 (2), 247–259.

O’Brien, S. (2018). Consumers Cough Up $5,400 a Year on Impulse Purchases. CNBC.com , February 23, Retrieved July 30, 2018 from https://www.cnbc.com/2018/02/23/consumers-cough-up-5400-a-year-on-impulse-purchases.html . Accessed 30 Jul 2018.

O'Guinn, T. C., & Faber, R. J. (1989). Compulsive buying: A phenomenological exploration. Journal of Consumer Research, 16 (2), 147–157.

Olsen, S. O., Tudoran, A. A., Honkanen, P., & Verplanken, B. (2016). Differences and similarities between impulse buying and variety seeking: A personality-based perspective. Psychology & Marketing, 33 (1), 36–47.

Orsingher, C., Valentini, S., & Angelis, M. (2009). A meta-analysis of satisfaction with complaint handling in services. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 38 (2), 169–186.

Palmatier, R. W., Dant, R. P., & Grewal, D. (2007). A comparative longitudinal analysis of theoretical perspectives of Interorganizational relationship performance. Journal of Marketing, 71 (4), 172–194.

Palmatier, R. W., Houston, M. B., & Hulland, J. (2018). Review articles: Purpose, process, and structure. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 46 (1), 1–8.

Parboteeah, D. V., Valacich, J. S., & Wells, J. D. (2009). The influence of website characteristics on a Consumer's urge to buy impulsively. Information Systems Research, 20 (1), 60–78.

Park, E. J., Kim, E. Y., Funches, V. M., & Foxx, W. (2012). Apparel product attributes, web browsing, and E-impulse buying on shopping websites. Journal of Business Research, 65 (11), 1583–1589.

Peck, J., & Childers, T. L. (2006). If I touch it I have to have it: Individual and environmental influences on impulse purchasing. Journal of Business Research, 59 (6), 765–769.

Peterson, R. A., & Brown, S. P. (2005). On the use of Beta coefficients in meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90 (1), 175–181.

Pick, D., & Eisend, M. (2014). Buyers’ perceived switching costs and switching: A meta-analytic assessment of their antecedents. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 42 (2), 186–204.

Punj, G. (2011). Impulse buying and variety seeking: Similarities and differences. Journal of Business Research, 64 (7), 745–748.

Ramanathan, S., & Menon, G. (2006). Time-varying effects of chronic hedonic goals on impulsive behavior. Journal of Marketing Research, 43 (4), 628–641.

Roggeveen, A. L., Nordfalt, J., & Grewal, D. (2016). Do digital displays enhance sales? Role of retail format and message content. Journal of Retailing, 92 (1), 122–131.

Rook, D. W. (1987). The buying impulse. Journal of Consumer Research, 14 (2), 189–199.

Rook, D. W., & Fisher, R. J. (1995). Normative influences on impulsive buying behavior. Journal of Consumer Research, 22 (3), 305–313.

Rook, D. W., & Gardner, M. P. (1993). In the mood: Impulsive Buyings’ antecedents. In J. Arnold-Costa & R. W. Belk (Eds.), Research in consumer behavior (pp. 1–28). Greenwich: JAI Press.

Rook, D. W., & Hoch, S. J. (1985). Consuming Impulses. In E. C. Hirschman & M. B. Holbrook (Eds.), Advances in consumer research (pp. 23–27). Chicago, IL: Association for Consumer Research, 23-27).

Rosenthal, R. (1979). The 'File drawer Problem' and tolerance for null results. Psychological Bulletin, 86 (3), 638–641.

Rostoks, L. (2003). Tapping into the shopper impulse. Canadian Grocer, 117 (October), 34–35.

Samaha, S. A., Beck, J. T., & Palmatier, R. W. (2014). The role of culture in international relationship marketing. Journal of Marketing, 78 (5), 78–98.

Sharma, A., & Stafford, T. F. (2000). The Effect of Retail Atmospherics on Customers' Perceptions of Salespeople and Customer Persuasion: An Empirical Investigation. Journal of Business Research, 49 (2), 183–191.

Sharma, P., Sivakumaran, B., & Marshall, R. (2010). Impulse buying and variety seeking: A trait-correlates perspective. Journal of Business Research, 63 (3), 276–283.

Sharma, P., Sivakumaran, B., & Marshall, R. (2014a). Looking beyond impulse buying: A cross-cultural and multi-domain investigation of consumer impulsiveness. European Journal of Marketing, 48 (5/6), 1159–1179.

Silvera, D. H., Lavack, A. M., & Kropp, F. (2008). Impulse buying: the role of affect, social influence, and subjective wellbeing. Journal of Consumer Marketing, 25 (1), 23–33.

Stilley, K. M., Inman, J. J., & Wakefield, K. L. (2010). Planning to make unplanned purchases? The role of in-store slack in budget deviation. Journal of Consumer Research, 37 (2), 264–278.

Sultan, A. J., Joireman, J., & Sprott, D. E. (2012). Building consumer self-control: The effect of self-control exercises on impulse buying urges. Marketing Letters, 23 (1), 61–72.

Thompson, E. R., & Prendergast, G. P. (2015). The influence of trait affect and the five-factor personality model on impulse buying. Personality and Individual Differences, 76 , 216–221.

Tice, D. M., Bratslavsky, E., & Baumeister, R. F. (2001). Emotional distress regulation takes precedence over impulse control: If you feel bad, do it! Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80 (1), 53–67.

Tifferet, S., & Herstein, R. (2012). Gender differences in brand commitment, impulse buying, and hedonic consumption. Journal of Product and Brand Management, 21 (3), 176–182.

Troisi, J. D., Christopher, A. C., & Marek, P. (2006). Materialism and money spending disposition as predictors of economic and personality variables. North American Journal of Psychology, 8 (3), 421–436.

Underhill, P. (2000). Why we buy: The science of shopping . New York: Simon and Schuster.

Van Trijp, H. C. M., & Steenkamp, J. E. M. (1992). Consumers' variety seeking tendency with respect to foods: Measurement and managerial implications. European Review of Agricultural Economics, 19 (2), 181–195.

Verhagen, T., & van Dolen, W. (2011). The influence of online store beliefs on consumer online impulse buying: A model and empirical application. Information and Management, 48 (8), 320–327.

Verplanken, B., & Herabadi, A. (2001). Individual differences in impulse buying tendency: Feeling and no thinking. European Journal of Personality, 15 (1), 71–83.

Verplanken, B., & Sato, A. (2011). The psychology of impulse buying: An integrative self-regulation approach. Journal of Consumer Policy, 34 (2), 197–210.

Verplanken, B., Herabadi, A. G., Perry, J. A., & Silvera, D. H. (2005). Consumer style and health: The role of impulsive buying in unhealthy eating. Psychology & Health, 20 (4), 429–441.

Vignoles, V. L., Regalia, C., Manzi, C., Golledge, J., & Scabini, E. (2006). Beyond self-esteem: Influence of multiple motives on identity construction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90 (2), 308–333.

Viswesvaran, C., & Ones, D. S. (1995). Theory testing: Combining psychometric meta-analysis and structural equations modeling. Personnel Psychology, 48 (4), 865–885.

Vohs, K. D., & Faber, R. J. (2007). Spent resources: Self-regulatory resource availability affects impulse buying. Journal of Consumer Research, 33 (4), 537–547.

Weinberg, P., & Gottwald, W. (1982). Impulsive consumer buying as a result of emotions. Journal of Business Research, 10 (1), 43–57.

Whiteside, S. P., & Lynam, D. R. (2001). The five factor model and impulsivity: Using a structural model of personality to understand impulsivity. Personality and Individual Differences, 30 (4), 669–689.

Wood, M. (1998). Socio-economic status, delay of gratification, and impulse buying. Journal of Economic Psychology, 19 (3), 295–320.

Zhang, Y., Winterich, K. P., & Mittal, V. (2010). Power distance belief and impulsive buying. Journal of Marketing Research, 47 (5), 945–954.

Zhou, L., & Wong, A. (2004). Consumer impulse buying and in-store stimuli in Chinese supermarkets. Journal of International Consumer Marketing, 16 (2), 37–53.

Zuckerman, M. (1994). Behavioral expressions and biosocial bases of sensation seeking . Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Download references

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

College of Business, Florida Atlantic University, 777 Glades Road, Boca Raton, FL, 33431, USA

Gopalkrishnan R. Iyer

Aston Business School, Aston University, Aston Triangle, Birmingham, B4 7ET, UK

Markus Blut

Durham University Business School, Durham University, Mill Hill Lane, Durham, DH1 3LB, UK

Sarah Hong Xiao

Babson College, 213 Malloy Hall, Babson Park, MA, 02457, USA

Dhruv Grewal

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Markus Blut .

Additional information

Publisher’s note.

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Mark Houston and John Hulland served as Special Issue Editors for this article.

Data were coded and analyzed by the second and third authors.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Iyer, G.R., Blut, M., Xiao, S.H. et al. Impulse buying: a meta-analytic review. J. of the Acad. Mark. Sci. 48 , 384–404 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11747-019-00670-w

Download citation

Received : 17 August 2018

Accepted : 06 June 2019

Published : 09 July 2019

Issue Date : May 2020

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/s11747-019-00670-w

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Meta-analysis
  • Impulse buying
  • Impulsivity
  • Mood states
  • Find a journal
  • Publish with us
  • Track your research

Academia.edu no longer supports Internet Explorer.

To browse Academia.edu and the wider internet faster and more securely, please take a few seconds to  upgrade your browser .

Enter the email address you signed up with and we'll email you a reset link.

  • We're Hiring!
  • Help Center

paper cover thumbnail

CONSUMERS' BUYING BEHAVIOR ON ONLINE SHOPPING: AN UTAUT AND LUM MODEL APPROACH

Profile image of Angelo Legaspi

Related Papers

Dr. Mohammad Naved Khan , MAMTA CHAWLA

Internet has gained status of as a dynamic commercial platform, more than a rich source of communication. It has intensified the complexities of the simple act of buying. “Google” has become the generic term for “searching information”. Traditional buying by individuals has taken the complex mixture of store, mall, television, internet, mobile- based shopping. Not only developed western-countries but even Asian countries, with poor infrastructure and low internet penetration rates, are equally adopting online buying. Indeed, a simple search combining the terms “online” and “buying” or “ shopping” results into more than 15000 results on any academic database source. A review of selected published work in the area of “online buying” reveals that a wide range of topics have been explored and a rich theoretical framework in the form of different models is inexistent. This paper aims to present a comprehensive framework of the relevant literature available in the field of online buying behavior, in the form of different theories, models and constructs; and research results based on them. Tradition 5-staged model of consumer behavior has different stages- need identification, information search, evaluation of alternatives, buying and post purchase evaluation. Additionally, for online buying behavior the stages involved in online buying can be divided into: attitude formation, intention, adoption and continuation with online buying. Most important factors that influence online buying: attitude, motivation, trust, risk, demographics, website etc. are widely researched and reported. “Internet adoption” is widely used as foundation framework to study “adoption of online buying”. Post adoption or continuation with online buying is the area which still needs substantiate research work. Current state of this emerging field offers the potential to identify areas that need attention for future researchers. Through review of online buying literature available, this paper offers theoretical basis to the academicians, practitioners and web-marketers. In addition, the clear understanding of the online buying behavior can provide the opportunities for designing new capabilities and strategies that would quench online buyers’ thrust on value.

research papers on buying

James-Ariel Sánchez-Alzate

This study explores the differences between buyers and non-buyers in the adoption of electronic purchase intention in Colombia. Based on the Unified Theory of Acceptance and Use of Technology (UTAUT), a theoretical model that includes a set of five variables is established: performance expectations, effort expectations, social influence, facilitating conditions, and risk. The empirical results obtained from a final sample of 1,836 surveys emphasize the importance of performance expectations for both groups. Social Influence is another determinant of electronic purchase as well. In addition, an exploratory study of the moderating effect of the educational level and socioeconomic status for each group was performed, finding strong evidence of the influence of these demographic variables, which suggests that, as a conclusion that makes a great contribution to this country, access to electronic shopping is strongly related to educational level and socioeconomic status.

International Journal of Advanced Computer Science and Applications

Dr Omkar Dastane

Abstract: E-Commerce tools have become a human needs everywhere and important not only to customers but to industry players. The intention to use E-Commerce tools among practitioners, especially in the Malaysian retail sector is not comprehensive as there are still many businesses choosing to use expensive traditional marketing. The research applies academic models and frameworks to the real life situation to develop a value proposition in the practical world by considering 11Street as the company under study and comparing it with Lazada as a leading competitor in the market. The objectives include identification of customers’ perception of a value for E-Commerce Businesses, followed by critical evaluation of existing value proposition of 11Street with Lazada to identify gap and finally to propose a new value proposition for 11street. This paper first identifies customer perceived value of E-Commerce followed by critical review of existing value proposition of 11Street and then comparing and contrasting with the leading player Lazada. By the end of this research, a new consumer value proposition proposal for 11Street proposed for consideration in matching with the Malaysian consumers’ value criteria.

Adamkolo M Ibrahim

This paper investigated factors that affect e-shopping acceptance among Nigerian students of tertiary institutions. The extended Unified Theory of Acceptance and Use of Technology (UTAUT2) model formulated by Venkatesh, Thong and Xu (2012) was adopted with some adjustments. The predictors of the model are performance expectancy, effort expectancy, social influence, facilitating conditions, hedonic motivation, price value and habit. Since the researchers wanted to study technology adoption in the context of service delivery quality, a review of relevant literature suggested designing an integrated model that would determine the influence of technology adoption expectancy and service quality variables on e-shopping adoption could yield comprehensive results. To achieve that, three key predictors from the Service Quality (SERVQUAL) model namely, reliability, responsiveness and empathy were integrated into the conceptual framework, which yielded the research model that was employed to determine the factor(s) that significantly affect adoption of e-shopping. The objectives of this study were to identify online shopping strategies of the e-tailers, to determine the students' perception of e-shopping and to determine the relationship between the predictors and e-shopping adoption. The integrated model, from which 10 hypotheses of this study were derived, measured the responses of 380 undergraduate (university) students. A pre-tested and validated 40-item questionnaire was administered to the respondents. The reliability coefficient of the items ranged from .755 to .876, which was high. A conclusion was drawn and some recommendations for future research were outlined.

Yusuf Lamidi

Adoption of Online shopping as being the latest purchasing channel in this decade of 24th century, particularly among the University students compared to the traditional shopping method as means of adoption of Technology Acceptance Method (TAM). Although, it is not clear what drives consumers to shop online and whether these numbers could be even increase if more attractive online stores were developed. This research seeks to critically explore and understand the social factors which influence the consumer intention to shop online, in student’s attitudinal behaviour. There has been an argument from different previous reviews concerning the objective and aim of this research (e.g. Davis, 1989; Monsuwe et al., 2004) but based on framework. So, therefore the study extend such framework which required re-examined and testing of the factors in order to make it more open for application in research context and also include students as consumers satisfaction to the definition of online shopping as recent factor to be consider. Mixed method strategy (quantitative and qualitative method) was adopted to explore and investigate validity and reliability of such social factors that drives consumers’ intention and attitude respectively of their demographic, role of product characteristics, situational factors, impact of previous experience and level of trust and satisfaction with web-stores. Based on the relevant literature review, therefore, the researcher carried out regression analysis test and thematic approach on the relative hypothesis. And the results of the regression analysis, manual statistical calculation and thematic approach (pattern) demonstrate that the social factors influence students’ intention and attitude toward online shopping with; (a) age and gender as basic factors, (b) product types like ticket/holiday booking and fashion, (c) ease of use, navigation and flexibility of web-site (d) usefulness of available information, effective for searching from various site and enjoyment from previous experience and, (d) quick delivery, problem solving with safety and security was found as satisfaction and trust of not just shopping presently but also using internet to shop in future.

suhana mohezar

Charles Dennis is a Senior Lecturer at Brunel University, London, UK. His teaching and research area is (e-)retail and consumer behaviour – the vital final link of the Marketing process – satisfying the end consumer. Charles is a Chartered Marketer and has been elected as a Fellow of the Chartered Institute of Marketing for work helping to modernise the teaching of the discipline.

TJPRC Publication

On-line shopping has seen an unprecedented growth globally, which also opens up new business avenues for stakeholders such as online retailers, e-commerce business platforms, banks and internet service operators. As per " India's Digital Leap–The Multi Trillion Dollar Opportunity " a report by Morgan Stanley, India's on-line shopping is expected to grow at an annual rate of 30% to reach about $200 billion for gross merchandise value by year 2026. Online shoppers in India are expected to grow from 14% of total internet base in 2016 to about 50% in 2026. Seeing this significant growth, the objective of the research paper is to identify and assess the factors influencing the adoption of online shopping and behavioral intention of online shoppers in Delhi/NCR, for which Exploratory, Descriptive and Causal research design has been adopted. This study had validated UTAUT model.

hemath weerasinghe

RELATED TOPICS

  •   We're Hiring!
  •   Help Center
  • Find new research papers in:
  • Health Sciences
  • Earth Sciences
  • Cognitive Science
  • Mathematics
  • Computer Science
  • Academia ©2024

research papers on buying

Professor of Methodology and Integrity, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam

Disclosure statement

Lex Bouter is the founding chair of the World Conferences on Research Integrity Foundation and co-chair of the 8th WCRI in Athens, 2-5 June 2024.

View all partners

An illustration of a magnifying glass poised over two wooden discs. Fake is written on one; real on the other

In the 1800s, British colonists in India set about trying to reduce the cobra population, which was making life and trade very difficult in Delhi. They began to pay a bounty for dead cobras. The strategy very quickly resulted in the widespread breeding of cobras for cash .

This danger of unintended consequences is sometimes referred to as the “ cobra effect ”. It can also be well summed up by Goodhardt’s Law , named after British economist Charles Goodhart. He stated that, when a measure becomes a target, it ceases to be a good measure.

The cobra effect has taken root in the world of research. The “publish or perish” culture, which values publications and citations above all, has resulted in its own myriad of “cobra breeding programmes”. That includes the widespread practice of questionable research practices, like playing up the impact of research findings to make work more attractive to publishers.

It’s also led to the rise of paper mills, criminal organisations that sell academic authorship. A report on the subject describes paper mills as (the)

process by which manufactured manuscripts are submitted to a journal for a fee on behalf of researchers with the purpose of providing an easy publication for them, or to offer authorship for sale.

These fake papers have serious consequences for research and its impact on society. Not all fake papers are retracted. And even those that are often still make their way into systematic literature reviews which are, in turn, used to draw up policy guidelines, clinical guidelines, and funding agendas.

How paper mills work

Paper mills rely on the desperation of researchers — often young, often overworked, often on the peripheries of academia struggling to overcome the high obstacles to entry — to fuel their business model.

They are frighteningly successful. The website of one such company based in Latvia advertises the publication of more than 12,650 articles since its launch in 2012. In an analysis of just two journals jointly conducted by the Committee on Publications Ethics and the International Association of Scientific, Technical and Medical Publishers, more than half of the 3440 article submissions over a two-year period were found to be fake.

It is estimated that all journals, irrespective of discipline, experience a steeply rising number of fake paper submissions. Currently the rate is about 2%. That may sound small. But, given the large and growing amount of scholarly publications it means that a lot of fake papers are published. Each of these can seriously damage patients, society or nature when applied in practice.

The fight against fake papers

Many individuals and organisations are fighting back against paper mills.

The scientific community is lucky enough to have several “fake paper detectives” who volunteer their time to root out fake papers from the literature. Elizabeth Bik , for instance, is a Dutch microbiologist turned science integrity consultant. She dedicates much of her time to searching the biomedical literature for manipulated photographic images or plagiarised text. There are others doing this work , too.

Organisations such as PubPeer and Retraction Watch also play vital roles in flagging fake papers and pressuring publishers to retract them.

These and other initiatives, like the STM Integrity Hub and United2Act , in which publishers collaborate with other stakeholders, are trying to make a difference.

But this is a deeply ingrained problem. The use of generative artificial intelligence like ChatGPT will help the detectives – but will also likely result in more fake papers which are now more easy to produce and more difficult or even impossible to detect.

Stop paying for dead cobras

They key to changing this culture is a switch in researcher assessment.

Researchers must be acknowledged and rewarded for responsible research practices: a focus on transparency and accountability, high quality teaching, good supervision, and excellent peer review. This will extend the scope of activities that yield “career points” and shift the emphasis of assessment from quantity to quality.

Fortunately, several initiatives and strategies already exist to focus on a balanced set of performance indicators that matter. The San Francisco Declaration on Research Assessment , established in 2012, calls on the research community to recognise and reward various research outputs, beyond just publication. The Hong Kong Principles , formulated and endorsed at the 6th World Conference in Research Integrity in 2019, encourage research evaluations that incentivise responsible research practices while minimise perverse incentives that drive practices like purchasing authorship or falsifying data.

These issues, as well as others related to protecting the integrity of research and building trust in it, will also be discussed during the 8th World Conference on Research Integrity in Athens, Greece in June this year.

Practices under the umbrella of “ Open Science ” will be pivotal to making the research process more transparent and researchers more accountable. Open Science is the umbrella term for a movement consisting of initiatives to make scholarly research more transparent and equitable, ranging from open access publication to citizen science.

Open Methods, for example, involves the pre-registration of a study design’s essential features before its start. A registered report containing the introduction and methods section is submitted to a journal before data collection starts. It is subsequently accepted or rejected based on the relevance of the research, as well as the methodology’s strength.

The added benefit of a registered report is that reviewer feedback on the methodology can still change the study methods, as the data collection hasn’t started. Research can then begin without pressure to achieve positive results, removing the incentive to tweak or falsify data.

Peer review

Peer reviewers are an important line of defence against the publication of fatally flawed or fake papers. In this system, quality assurance of a paper is done on a completely voluntary and often anonymous basis by an expert in the relevant field or subject.

However, the person doing the review work receives no credit or reward. It’s crucial that this sort of “invisible” work in academia be recognised, celebrated and included among the criteria for promotion. This can contribute substantially to detecting questionable research practices (or worse) before publication.

It will incentivise good peer review, so fewer suspect articles pass through the process, and it will also open more paths to success in academia – thus breaking up the toxic publish-or-perish culture.

This article is based on a presentation given by the lead author at Stellenbosch University, South Africa on 12 February 2024. Natalie Simon, a communications consultant specialising in research who is part of the communications team for the 8th World Conference on Research Integrity and is also currently completing an MPhil in Science and Technology Studies at Stellenbosch University, co-authored this article.

  • Academic journals
  • Research integrity
  • Academic research
  • Publish or perish
  • Fake journals
  • Paper mills
  • Open Science movement

research papers on buying

School of Social Sciences – Academic appointment opportunities

research papers on buying

Union Organiser (part-time 0.8)

research papers on buying

Director, Indigenous Education Research Centre

research papers on buying

Communications Specialist

research papers on buying

AUSTRALIAN PESTICIDES AND VETERINARY MEDICINES AUTHORITY (APVMA) BOARD CHAIR

Tax Policy and Investment in a Global Economy

We evaluate the 2017 Tax Cuts and Jobs Act. Combining reduced-form estimates from tax data with a global investment model, we estimate responses, identify parameters, and conduct counterfactuals. Domestic investment of firms with the mean tax change increases 20% versus a no-change baseline. Due to novel foreign incentives, foreign capital of U.S. multinationals rises substantially. These incentives also boost domestic investment, indicating complementarity between domestic and foreign capital. In the model, the long-run effect on domestic capital in general equilibrium is 7% and the tax revenue feedback from growth offsets only 2p.p. of the direct cost of 41% of pre-TCJA corporate revenue.

We thank Agustin Barboza, Emily Bjorkman, Walker Lewis, Anh-Huy Nguyen, Shivani Pandey, Sarah Robinson, Francesco Ruggieri, Sam Thorpe, and Caleb Wroblewski for excellent research assistance; our discussants Eyal Argov, Steven Bond, Manon François, Andrea Lanteri, and Jason Furman; and seminar and conference participants for comments, ideas, and help with data. We thank Michael Caballero, Anne Moore, and Laura Power for insights on multinational tax data and Tom Winberry for helpful discussions about his adjustment cost estimates. Chodorow-Reich gratefully acknowledges support from the Ferrante Fund and Chae fund at Harvard University. Zwick gratefully acknowledges financial support from the Booth School of Business at the University of Chicago. Zidar thanks the NSF for support under grant no. 1752431. Disclaimer: All data work for this project involving confidential taxpayer information was done at IRS facilities, on IRS computers, and at no time was confidential taxpayer data ever outside of the IRS computing environment. The views expressed herein are those of the authors and do not necessarily represent the views of the IRS, the U.S. Department of the Treasury, or the National Bureau of Economic Research. All results have been reviewed to ensure that no confidential information is disclosed. The model-implied revenue estimates are not revenue estimates of the TCJA.

MARC RIS BibTeΧ

Download Citation Data

Conferences

More from nber.

In addition to working papers , the NBER disseminates affiliates’ latest findings through a range of free periodicals — the NBER Reporter , the NBER Digest , the Bulletin on Retirement and Disability , the Bulletin on Health , and the Bulletin on Entrepreneurship  — as well as online conference reports , video lectures , and interviews .

15th Annual Feldstein Lecture, Mario Draghi, "The Next Flight of the Bumblebee: The Path to Common Fiscal Policy in the Eurozone cover slide

research papers on buying

Buy a research paper online

Simplify your research paper creation with professional service

Our price starting at just $11.7 per page written by our top-level writers.

Trusted by 1,5M+ happy customers

Buy research paper online

Buy research papers from gurus of writing

We introduce you to a team of skilled writers once you buy research papers online.

Neat W.

Reasons to buy a research paper online with EssayPro

Years of writing experience.

You'll have access to top writers from US and Canada.

Our minimal deadline delivery

Our service delivers speedy work to meet your deadlines.

Plagiarism-free content

Students always get fully original content and a free plagiarism report along with it.

Subjects and paper types

Service from us is possible for every academic subject. Get all assignments from one place.

Per page - our starting price

Our affordable rates start at only $10.80 per page of writing.

Choose us for affordable rates to buy research paper online

Filter through customer feedback reviews to gain insight from other students who work with us.

How to make order

1. provide instructions for your paper.

To buy a research paper, provide useful details such as the due date, paper length, and any other important information. You can also attach files.

2. Select your expert writer

Order research paper from our service and pick one of our qualified writers. Read their customer reviews and bio to learn more about their experience.

3. Get an order

After you will receive an email when your order is complete. You will then be able to review it before paying.

research papers on buying

We offer many benefits for students

Unlimited revisions.

Feel free to ask your writer for unlimited corrections within 30 days of your order.

Wait to pay

You can first review your research paper and pay only when you're 100% satisfied with the outcome purchasing research papers from us.

High quality services

We have just the right set of professionals to help you cover any academic subject or topic necessary.

We protect privacy

Students trust us as they know that we protect their privacy by having an encrypted website.

Frequently asked questions

How can i buy a research paper, why should i buy a research paper, am i buying a research paper from writers who follow directions, what are the deadlines for purchasing research papers, will my research paper be original, what features are included when buying research paper online, relieve your stress - buy research paper from essaypro, why should i choose your service.

EssayPro is a trustworthy service with the highest quality writers on the internet. Our research paper writer helps students like you relax when they feel the most overwhelmed. When you’re faced with a challenging essay or task, our writing team is here to help. Students use our service when they need help balancing work and school or simply need a break to practice self-care. No matter why you come to us, when you pay for research papers through us ensure you’ll get the break you deserve.

As students search for services for research papers to buy online, they find that we have the best rankings. We help you boost your future opportunities and help you succeed! We offer everything students need, from the ability to choose their own writer to 24/7 customer support to help them each step of the way. The mission of our team is to ensure that every learner can have access to the best writing help when they need it. That’s why we’re here to handle any type of paper, from research papers for sale to term papers. We’re equipped to handle all types of academic essays in every field or subject!When you’re feeling stressed, you can rely on us for quick, reliable, and affordable online writing help. Of course, our friendly customer support team is always available to answer any questions you may have about our process. From start to finish, students are fully supported when they use our service for academic help.

Choose an expert to purchase research paper orders from

What author will be work with me if I could research paper to buy? You’ll have access to a wide selection of top-notch writers. Our large pool of qualified essay writers is always here, ready to help you create the best essay possible. When you ready to place an order, we give you the opportunity to select your own writer. Browse through the description and bios of each of our writers to find an author who you feel is best suited for your needs.

To select your writer, you can filter specialists by their fields of expertise and experience. This will help you ensure that your expert is well-equipped to write for the academic field you need. Our service makes the process of finding a simple. Buy an original research paper easy! In addition to a short description of each writer and years of experience, you’ll also be able to view their past customer reviews, overall rating, and success rates. Using all of this information will ensure you pick the perfect writer. If you’re having trouble selecting a writer for the job, we’re here to help. Our customer support team is happy to lend a helping hand, 24/7. Simply send us a message and let us know you need help selecting an expert writer for the job. We will guide you through the selection process to help you find the most suitable author for research papers. At EssayPro, our goal is to make your stay with us stress-free. That’s why we’re here to fully satisfy any your request.

Buying a research paper and get help from an expert writer

Students are often overwhelmed and overworked during the academic semester. If you’re feeling like it’s hard to catch up with your schoolwork, we are here to help. You’re in the right place for academic help that is simple and stress-free. When you buy a research paper through us, one of our qualified writers will help you each step of the way. The process is easy. Simply select your ideal writer, give them the requirements for your paper, and let them take over. You’ll have a chance to catch up on your other responsibilities while we write a high-quality paper for you.

Buying a research paper online through EssayPro comes with a 100% guarantee of authentic and original work. We recommend that you submit a piece of your past writing so your writer can get an idea of your voice and writing style. This will help them to match the new essay to sound just like you. Every research paper completed by our expert writers is run through plagiarism software to help ensure the work is 100% authentic. If desired, you may request a copy of the authenticity report with your order.

Buy from our super writers and you can sit back, relax, and breathe! Stressed-out students come to us for the support they need. We take your privacy seriously too and will never disclose your personal information to third parties. Using EssayPro is entirely anonymous. We’re the service you can trust. Not to mention, our services are affordable for students like you. Contact us when you feel overwhelmed. We guarantee you’ll be glad you did.

What if I am not happy after buying research paper?

As a trusted service, we take pride in making each of our customers happy. We have been in the market for over 7 years, so we know what our clients are looking for. Although it is rare that customers are unhappy with our services, we acknowledge that it may happen occasionally.

We offer assistance to students looking to buy cheap research paper online , striving for excellence with every order. If you are unhappy we will make every adjustment and correction necessary until you are fully satisfied with our work. For complex orders, you may have several requirements that must be met by our writers. Our team of professional writers is always open to making as many rounds of edits as necessary. Within 30 days of your order, you can request unlimited corrections from your writer. Simply let your writer know what needs to be changed and they will be happy to help. In order to request edits, you’ll need to message the writer and explain your concerns. The writer, along with our quality assurance team, will step in to ensure your research papers is corrected promptly. We want every student who orders through our service to love their essays. If you are still unhappy with your essay after corrections, we will accommodate a refund. Students can request a full refund within 30 days of their order. However, this is extremely rare as most students are 100% satisfied with their essays after edits are completed. We take every effort to make sure the students who order research papers from us are never let down.

Still thinking "I need someone to write my paper ?" feel free to contact us!

research papers on buying

  • Growth Marketing
  • Content Strategy
  • SEO Strategy
  • Health Care
  • Guide to Growth Marketing
  • Guide to Digital PR
  • SEO Strategy: The Ultimate Guide
  • Website Content Strategy Guide

research papers on buying

Buying Research Papers: Is It Legit to Order Them Online?

research papers on buying

Whenever you think of giving up on your assignment and buying it from freelance writers, you can almost feel like the word “plagiarist” is being written all over your face. However, buying papers online is not always about cheating. If you are seeking academic help, you need someone competent enough to deal with your paper, and with academic credentials to pull studies from places like NCBI. Today you can get this help quickly and confidentially. It is one of the many businesses we use, and there is nothing wrong about asking for academic help online.

However, there are a few factors to take into account when searching for a writer to complete your assignment. In this article, we will explain why academic help services deserve to exist and how you can use them legitimately.

Why have so many research writing websites appeared on the web?

Studying is a hard job, and with every year, more and more students consider find it difficult to meet study demands. International kids can struggle to assimilate and overcome the language barrier. American students, on the other hand, are often occupied with so many extra-schooling activities that they barely have time to take classes. Still, all of them need good grades to complete their degrees successfully. This is a good reason to search for academic writing services online.

Years ago, the essay writing business looked more like a fraudulent endeavor. Clients never knew what they would receive as they paid for their papers in advance. Companies used to resell essays available on the web and made money producing nothing in particular. Today things have changed in this field, and many websites have proven their credibility. Trustworthy companies have withstood the test of time, as they have hired competent writers and organized smart customer support.

Priceless time vs. academic integrity: what wins?

Can you really hope that academic help services will not undermine the academic integrity of schools all over the US? Technically, many paper writing services claim themselves to be legitimate, and it’s hard to disprove that. The academic assignments they sell are meant to be a sample for students to write their own papers well. Customers are not supposed to submit essays completed by professional writers. Therefore, cheating does not come into view.

Still, no one can control whether students submit the papers they buy or not. Now it’s time for customers to choose whether they support the academic integrity of their institution or the value of their own time above that. We could have a long debate on the ethics of cheating here but your choice is the only thing that matters.

Getting your paper done by someone else is not always cheating

If you have always thought that academic help companies will do no good for your performance, you should definitely reconsider that. Freelance writers give you an opportunity to learn and improve your own skills so that your own papers can improve with other assignment. Take it as a simple business transaction: you are purchasing an example of what your paper should look like. Seeking extra materials for your homework is completely legitimate and it does not make you a bad person. It can be your way to improve yourself.

Sometimes we get so demanding of ourselves that we forget to make our life simpler. It would be much more comfortable to ask a freelancer for help and get out of this research-paper crisis. You are not so much cheating with this assignment as making your life easier.

One should note however that certain professors or schools may have regulations or standards preventing the submission of these kinds of papers which should definitely be taken into account by the student.

How to buy a research paper from a reliable company

When writing a research paper , you will need a good sample with the necessary structure, formatting, and citations. If you buy it from a credible academic help company, you will get an original research paper written according to your demands. Such an example will cut your research time and give you ideas on what to put in your assignment.

While searching for a reliable custom writing service, pay attention to how long the company has been functioning in the market and who the writers are. Also, read testimonials provided by independent services, like Trustpilot . We recommend CustomWritings as the right place to buy research papers online, customized to your academic needs. They have a wide range of disciplines to choose from and plenty of writers competent in different fields. By using this service, you can relieve your student pain of writing overly long or overly complex assignments.

Today, it’s fast and easy to purchase your assignment online. You just fill in the order form, pay for the writer’s job, and wait for your research paper to arrive within the deadline. Websites usually organize a timely delivery, as they cherish their customers and want them to place more orders. That is why you can feel confident about your paper and enjoy getting legitimate academic help from custom writing websites.

Help | Advanced Search

Computer Science > Computer Vision and Pattern Recognition

Title: emo: emote portrait alive -- generating expressive portrait videos with audio2video diffusion model under weak conditions.

Abstract: In this work, we tackle the challenge of enhancing the realism and expressiveness in talking head video generation by focusing on the dynamic and nuanced relationship between audio cues and facial movements. We identify the limitations of traditional techniques that often fail to capture the full spectrum of human expressions and the uniqueness of individual facial styles. To address these issues, we propose EMO, a novel framework that utilizes a direct audio-to-video synthesis approach, bypassing the need for intermediate 3D models or facial landmarks. Our method ensures seamless frame transitions and consistent identity preservation throughout the video, resulting in highly expressive and lifelike animations. Experimental results demonsrate that EMO is able to produce not only convincing speaking videos but also singing videos in various styles, significantly outperforming existing state-of-the-art methodologies in terms of expressiveness and realism.

Submission history

Access paper:.

  • Download PDF
  • HTML (experimental)
  • Other Formats

license icon

References & Citations

  • Google Scholar
  • Semantic Scholar

BibTeX formatted citation

BibSonomy logo

Bibliographic and Citation Tools

Code, data and media associated with this article, recommenders and search tools.

  • Institution

arXivLabs: experimental projects with community collaborators

arXivLabs is a framework that allows collaborators to develop and share new arXiv features directly on our website.

Both individuals and organizations that work with arXivLabs have embraced and accepted our values of openness, community, excellence, and user data privacy. arXiv is committed to these values and only works with partners that adhere to them.

Have an idea for a project that will add value for arXiv's community? Learn more about arXivLabs .

Cart

  • SUGGESTED TOPICS
  • The Magazine
  • Newsletters
  • Managing Yourself
  • Managing Teams
  • Work-life Balance
  • The Big Idea
  • Data & Visuals
  • Reading Lists
  • Case Selections
  • HBR Learning
  • Topic Feeds
  • Account Settings
  • Email Preferences

When New Hires Get Paid More, Top Performers Resign First

  • Andrea Derler,
  • Peter Bamberger,
  • Manda Winlaw,
  • Cuthbert Chow

research papers on buying

Research shows that unaddressed pay gaps will push veteran talent to find new jobs.

To attract new talent, employers often offer new hires higher wages than existing employees. But today, a combination of regulatory changes and technological advances have dramatically increased pay transparency in many sectors, making employees increasingly aware of these pay disparities. How do existing employees (and especially top performers) react to these higher-paid new hires? And how can organizations mitigate the associated risks? The authors’ recent research shows that unless employers adjust existing employees’ wages soon after making a new hire, employees tend to resign — and that top performers tend to resign faster than others. As such, employers should be aware of the impact hiring higher-paid external talent can have on their teams, conduct regular pay equity analyses to ensure that any disparities are fully explainable, and develop the agility necessary to adjust wages as soon as any inequities are identified.

To attract top talent, employers often pay new hires more than they pay existing employees in equivalent roles. This isn’t new . But today, regulatory changes and technological advances have dramatically increased pay transparency in many sectors, making employees more aware of these pay disparities. Moreover, data from the U.S. Chamber of Commerce indicates that the workforce is expected to shrink in 2024, while a global survey of more than 30,000 employees found that salaries are expected to increase by an average of 4% in 2024, suggesting that these pay gaps will likely continue to expand.

  • AD Andrea Derler is Visier’s principal of research and value, where she collaborates with Visier’s team of data scientists, people analytics experts, as well as HR professionals to help produce practical, data-driven insights for organizations. Visier is a Canadian-based people analytics technology provider, which maintains a database of more than 15 million employee records from over 15,000 companies globally.
  • PB Peter Bamberger is a professor of organizational behavior and the head of the organizational behavior department at the Coller School of Management, Tel Aviv University. He is the former editor in chief of Academy of Management Discoveries and currently serves as the President-Elect of the Academy of Management, as well as the research director of the Smithers Institute of Cornell University’s ILR School. He is a world-leading expert on compensation strategy and pay communication. His work has been cited over 15,000 times.
  • MW Manda Winlaw is a data science manager at Visier, contributing to research and developing data products for the Visier app. She has worked as a data scientist in a number of different fields and strongly believes in using data to drive better decisions for all people.
  • CC Cuthbert Chow , MDS, BBA, and LLB, works in Visier’s publications focus area as a data scientist co-op. Cuthbert’s primary role is discovering novel and industry-relevant insights within Visier’s rich community data. Cuthbert holds a master’s in data science and bachelor’s in law and business.

Partner Center

  • Skip to main content
  • Keyboard shortcuts for audio player

Shots - Health News

  • Your Health
  • Treatments & Tests
  • Health Inc.

Public Health

Meet the public health researchers trying to rein in america's gun violence crisis.

Christine Spolar

research papers on buying

A digital illustration of a circle of hands extending from the edge of the image, each holding a sheet of paper. The papers overlap in the center and, like a puzzle, come together to reveal a drawing of a handgun. Oona Tempest/KFF Health News hide caption

A digital illustration of a circle of hands extending from the edge of the image, each holding a sheet of paper. The papers overlap in the center and, like a puzzle, come together to reveal a drawing of a handgun.

Gun violence has exploded across the U.S. in recent years — from mass shootings at concerts and supermarkets to school fights settled with a bullet after the last bell.

Nearly every day of 2024 so far has brought more violence. On Feb. 14, gunfire at the Super Bowl parade in Kansas City, Missouri, killed one woman and wounded 22 other people. Most events draw little attention — while the injuries and toll pile up.

Gun violence is among America's most deadly and costly public health crises. But unlike other big killers — diseases like cancer and HIV or dangers like automobile crashes and cigarettes — sparse federal money goes to studying or preventing it.

That's because of a one-sentence amendment tucked into the 1996 Congressional budget bill: "None of the funds made available for injury prevention and control at the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention may be used to advocate or promote gun control."

Its author was Jay Dickey, an Arkansas Republican who called himself the "point man" for the National Rifle Association on Capitol Hill. And for nearly 25 years the amendment was perceived as a threat to, and all but paralyzed, the CDC's support and study of gun violence.

Goats and Soda

How the u.s. gun violence death rate compares with the rest of the world.

Even so, a small group of academics have toiled to document how gun violence courses through American communities with vast and tragic outcomes. Their research provides some light as officials and communities develop policies mostly in the dark.

It has also inspired a fresh generation of researchers to enter the field – people who grew up with mass shootings and are now determined to investigate harm from firearms. There is momentum now, in a time of rising gun injury and death, to know more.

The reality is stark:

Gun sales reached record levels in 2019 and 2020. Shootings soared. In 2021, for the second year , more people died from gun incidents — 48,830 — than in any year on record, according to a Johns Hopkins University analysis of CDC data. Guns became the leading cause of death for children and teens. Suicides accounted for more than half of those deaths, and homicides were linked to 4 in 10.

Gun deaths hit their highest level ever in 2021, with 1 person dead every 11 minutes

Gun deaths hit their highest level ever in 2021, with 1 person dead every 11 minutes

Black people are nearly 14 times as likely to die from firearm violence as white people — and guns were responsible for half of all deaths of Black teens ages 15 to 19 in 2021, the data showed.

Harvard research published in JAMA in 2022 estimated gun injuries translate into economic losses of $557 billion annually , or 2.6% of the U.S. gross domestic product.

With gun violence touching nearly every corner of the country, surveys show that Americans — whatever their political affiliation or whether they own guns or not — support policies that could reduce violence .

Quashing a quest for knowledge

It is no secret that many strategies for reducing harm from guns proposed today — from school metal detectors to enhanced policing, to the optimal timing and manner of safely storing guns, to restrictions on gun sales — have limited scientific ballast because of a lack of data.

It could have been otherwise.

U.S. firearm production surged in the late 1980s , flooding communities with more than 200 million weapons . In that era, Mark Rosenberg was the founding director of the CDC's National Center for Injury Prevention and Control and his agency, over time, was pivotal in helping to fund research on gun violence and public health.

research papers on buying

Mark Rosenberg, one of the nation's top authorities on gun violence and public health, was the founding director of the National Center for Injury Prevention and Control at the CDC. Oona Tempest/KFF Health News hide caption

Rosenberg thought then that gun violence could go the way of car crashes. The federal government spent $200 million a year on research to redesign roadways and cars beginning in the 1970s, he said, and had seen death rates plummeted.

"We said, 'Why can't we do this with gun violence?'" Rosenberg said. "They figured out how to get rid of car crashes — but not cars. Why can't we do the same thing when it comes to guns?"

The Dickey Amendment sidelined that dream.

A study published in 1993 concluded that "guns kept in the home are associated with an increase in the risk of homicide," a finding on risk factors that prompted an uproar in conservative political circles. To newly elected representatives in the midterm "Republican Revolution" of 1994, the research was a swipe at gun rights. The NRA stepped up lobbying, and Congress passed what's known as the Dickey Amendment in 1996.

Some Democrats, such as the influential John Dingell of Michigan (a onetime NRA board member who received the group's " legislative achievement award "), would join the cause. Dingell proposed his own bills, detailed last summer by The New York Times .

Under heavy political pressure, the CDC ousted Rosenberg in 1999. Soon after, some CDC administrators began alerting the NRA to research before publication.

"It was clearly related to the work we were doing on gun violence prevention," Rosenberg, now 78, said of his job loss. "It was a shock."

Gun researchers who persevered

The quarter-century spending gap has left a paucity of data about the scope of gun violence's health effects: Who is shot and why? What motivates the violence? With what guns? What are the injuries? Can suicides, on the rise from gunfire, be reduced or prevented with safeguards? Does drug and alcohol use increase the chances of harm? Could gun safeguards reduce domestic violence? Ultimately, what works and what does not to prevent shootings?

If researchers say they "lost a generation" of knowledge about gun violence, then American families lost even more, with millions of lives cut short and a legacy of trauma passed down through generations.

research papers on buying

Rebecca Cunningham, the vice president of research at the University of Michigan and an emergency medical doctor, organized a national conference last fall on the prevention of firearm harm that drew more than 750 academics and public health, law, and criminal justice experts. "You can feel momentum" for change, she says. Oona Tempest/KFF Health News hide caption

Imagine if cancer research had been halted in 1996 — many tumors that are now eminently treatable might still be lethal. "It's like cancer," said Rebecca Cunningham, vice president for research at the University of Michigan, an academic who has kept the thread of gun research going all these years. "There may be 50 kinds of cancer, and there are preventions for all of them. Firearm violence has many different routes, and it will require different kinds of science and approaches."

Cunningham is one of a small group of like-minded researchers from universities across the United States, who refused to let go of investigating a growing public health risk, and they pushed ahead without government funds.

Garen Wintemute has spent about $2.45 million of his money to support seminal research at the University of California-Davis. With state and private funding, he created a violence prevention program in California, a leader in firearm studies. He has documented an unprecedented increase in gun sales since 2020 — about 15 million transactions more than expected based on previous sales data.

research papers on buying

Daniel Webster, a Johns Hopkins University researcher, has focused on teenagers and guns. Oona Tempest/KFF Health News hide caption

Daniel Webster at Johns Hopkins University focused on teenagers and guns — particularly access and suicides — and found that local police who coped with gun risks daily were willing to collaborate. He secured grants, even from the CDC, with carefully phrased proposals that avoided the word "guns," to study community violence.

At Duke University, Philip J. Cook explored the underground gun market, interviewing people incarcerated in Chicago jails and compiling pivotal social science research on how guns are bought, sold, and traded.

David Hemenway , an economist and public policy professor at Harvard, worked on the national pilot to document violent deaths — knowing most gun deaths would be recorded that way — because, he said, "if you don't have good data, you don't have nothin'."

Hemenway, writing in the journal Nature in 2017, found a 30% rise in gun suicides over the preceding decade and nearly a 20% rise in gun murders from 2014 to 2015. The data was alarming and so was the lack of preventive know-how, he wrote. "The US government, at the behest of the gun lobby, limits the collection of data, prevents researchers from obtaining much of the data that are collected and severely restricts the funds available for research on guns," he wrote. "Policymakers are essentially flying blind."

research papers on buying

David Hemenway, a Harvard economist and public policy professor, anchored the work that led to the most ambitious database of U.S. gun deaths today. Oona Tempest/KFF Health News hide caption

His work helped create the most ambitious database of U.S. gun deaths today — the National Violent Death Reporting System . Funded in 1999 by private foundations, researchers were able to start understanding gun deaths by compiling data on all violent deaths from health department, police, and crime records in several states. The CDC took over the system and eventually rolled in data from all 50 states.

Still, no federal database of nonfatal gun injuries exists. So the government would record one death from the Super Bowl parade shooting, and the 22 people with gunshot injuries remain uncounted — along with many thousands of others over decades.

Philanthropy has supported research that Congress would not. The Joyce Foundation in Chicago funded the bulk of the grants, with more than $33 million since the 1990s. Arnold Ventures ' philanthropy and the Robert Wood Johnson Foundation have added millions more, as has Michael Bloomberg, the politician and media company owner. The Rand Corp. , which keeps a tab of ongoing research, finds states increasingly are stepping up.

Timothy Daly, a Joyce Foundation program director, said he remembers when the field of gun harm was described by some as a "desert." "There was no federal funding. There was slim private funding," he said. "Young people would ask themselves: 'Why would I go into that?'"

Research published in JAMA in 2017 found gun violence "was the least-researched" among leading causes of death. Looking at mortality rates over a decade, gun violence killed about as many people as sepsis, the data showed. If funded at the same rate, gun violence would have been expected to receive $1.4 billion in research funds. Instead, it received $22 million from across all U.S. government agencies.

There is no way to know what the firearm mortality or injury rate would be today had there been more federal support for strategies to contain it.

A reckoning and new push for research

As gun violence escalated to once unthinkable levels, Congressman Dickey came to regret his role in stanching research and became friends with Rosenberg. They wrote a pivotal Washington Post op-ed about the need for gun injury prevention studies. In 2016, they delivered a letter supporting the creation of the California Firearm Violence Research Center.

Both men, they emphasized, were NRA members and agreed on two principles: "One goal must be to protect the Second-Amendment rights of law-abiding gun owners; the other goal, to reduce gun violence."

Dickey died in 2017, and Rosenberg has only kind words for him. "I did not blame Jay at all for what happened," he said. The CDC was "under pressure from Congress to get rid of our gun research."

As alarm over gun fatality statistics from diverse sectors of the nation — scientists, politicians, and law enforcement — has grown, research in the field is finally gaining a foothold.

Even Congress, noting the Dickey Amendment was not an all-out ban, appropriated $25 million for gun research in late 2019, split between the CDC — whose imperative is to research public health issues — and the National Institutes of Health. It's a drop in the bucket compared with what was spent on car crashes, and it's not assured. House Republicans this winter have pushed an amendment to once again cut federal funding for CDC gun research.

Still it's a start. And with growing interest in the field, the torch has passed to the next generation of researchers.

In November, Cunningham helped organize a national conference on the prevention of firearm-related harm. More than 750 academics and professionals in public health, law, and criminal justice met in Chicago for hundreds of presentations. A similar event in 2019, the first in 20 years, drew just a few dozen presentations.

"You can feel momentum," Cunningham said at the conference, reflecting on the research underway. "There's a momentum to propel a whole series of evidence-based change — in the same way we have addressed other health problems."

During a congressional hearing weeks later , Yale University School of Public Health Dean Megan L. Ranney bluntly described the rising number of gun deaths — noting the overwhelming number of suicides — as a warning for lawmakers. "We are turning into a nation of traumatized survivors," she said, urging their support for better data and research on risk factors.

research papers on buying

Cassandra Crifasi, co-director of the Johns Hopkins Center for Gun Violence Solutions, was in high school when the Columbine massacre shook the country. Oona Tempest/KFF Health News hide caption

Cassandra Crifasi, 41, was a high school sophomore when the Columbine massacre outside Littleton, Colorado, shook the country. She recently succeeded Webster, her mentor and research partner , as co-director of the Johns Hopkins Center for Gun Violence Solutions.

Crifasi has spent much of her career evaluating risk factors in gun use, including collaborative studies with Baltimore police and the city to reduce violence.

Raised in Washington state, Crifasi said she never considered required training in firearms an affront to the Second Amendment. She owns guns. In her family, which hunted, it was a matter of responsibility.

"We all learned to hunt. There are rules to follow. Maybe we should have everybody who wants to have a gun to do that," she said.

Crifasi pointed to the 2018 shooting at Marjory Stoneman Douglas High in Parkland, Florida — which left 17 dead and 17 injured — as a turning point. Students and their parents took "a page out of Mothers Against Drunk Driving — showing up, testifying, being in the gallery where laws are made," she said.

"People started to shift and started to think: This is not a third rail in politics. This is not a third rail in research," Crifasi said.

research papers on buying

Shani Buggs, a lead investigator at the California Firearm Violence Research Center, has studied anxiety and depression among young people who live in neighborhoods with gun violence. Oona Tempest/KFF Health News hide caption

Shani Buggs worked in corporate management before she arrived at Johns Hopkins to pursue a master's in public health. It was summer 2012, and a gunman killed 12 moviegoers at a midnight showing of "The Dark Knight Rises" in Aurora, Colorado. The town's pain led the national news, and "rightfully so," Buggs said. "But I was in Baltimore, in East Baltimore, where there were shootings happening that weren't even consistently making the local news."

Now violence "that once was considered out of bounds, out of balance — it is more and more common," said Buggs who recently joined the California Firearm Violence Research Center as a lead investigator.

Buggs' research has examined anxiety and depression among youths who live in neighborhoods with gun violence — and notes that firearm suicide rates too have drastically increased among Black children and adolescents.

There is a trauma from hearing gunshots and seeing gun injuries, and daily life can be a thrum of risk in vulnerable communities, notably those largely populated by Black and Hispanic people, Buggs said. Last year, Buggs organized the Black and Brown Collective with a core group of about two dozen scientists committed to contextualizing studies on gun violence.

"The people most impacted by the gun violence we usually hear about in America look like our families," she said of the collective.

"They are not resilient. People are just surviving," Buggs said. "We need way more money to research and to understand and address the complexity of the problem."

KFF Health News , formerly known as Kaiser Health News (KHN), is a national newsroom that produces in-depth journalism about health issues and is one of the core operating programs at KFF — the independent source for health policy research, polling, and journalism.

  • gun violence research
  • gun violence prevention
  • gun violence

U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings
  • Advanced Search
  • Journal List
  • Front Psychol

Factors Affecting Impulse Buying Behavior of Consumers

In recent years, the study of consumer behavior has been marked by significant changes, mainly in decision-making process and consequently in the influences of purchase intention (Stankevich, 2017 ).

The markets are different and characterized by an increased competition, as well a constant innovation in products and services available and a greater number of companies in the same market. In this scenario it is essential to know the consumer well (Varadarajan, 2020 ). It is through the analysis of the factors that have a direct impact on consumer behavior that it is possible to innovate and meet their expectations. This research is essential for marketers to be able to improve their campaigns and reach the target audience more effectively (Ding et al., 2020 ).

Consumer behavior refers to the activities directly involved in obtaining products /services, so it includes the decision-making processes that precede and succeed these actions. Thus, it appears that the advertising message can cause a certain psychological influence that motivates individuals to desire and, consequently, buy a certain product/service (Wertenbroch et al., 2020 ).

Studies developed by Meena ( 2018 ) show that from a young age one begins to have a preference for one product/service over another, as we are confronted with various commercial stimuli that shape our choices. The sales promotion has become one of the most powerful tools to change the perception of buyers and has a significant impact on their purchase decision (Khan et al., 2019 ). Advertising has a great capacity to influence and persuade, and even the most innocuous, can cause changes in behavior that affect the consumer's purchase intention. Falebita et al. ( 2020 ) consider this influence predominantly positive, as shown by about 84.0% of the total number of articles reviewed in the study developed by these authors.

Kumar et al. ( 2020 ) add that psychological factors have a strong implication in the purchase decision, as we easily find people who, after having purchased a product/ service, wonder about the reason why they did it. It is essential to understand the mental triggers behind the purchase decision process, which is why consumer psychology is related to marketing strategies (Ding et al., 2020 ). It is not uncommon for the two areas to use the same models to explain consumer behavior and the reasons that trigger impulse purchases. Consumers are attracted by advertising and the messages it conveys, which is reflected in their behavior and purchase intentions (Varadarajan, 2020 ).

Impulse buying has been studied from several perspectives, namely: (i) rational processes; (ii) emotional resources; (iii) the cognitive currents arising from the theory of social judgment; (iv) persuasive communication; (v) and the effects of advertising on consumer behavior (Malter et al., 2020 ).

The causes of impulsive behavior are triggered by an irresistible force to buy and an inability to evaluate its consequences. Despite being aware of the negative effects of buying, there is an enormous desire to immediately satisfy your most pressing needs (Meena, 2018 ).

The importance of impulse buying in consumer behavior has been studied since the 1940's, since it represents between 40.0 and 80.0% of all purchases. This type of purchase obeys non-rational reasons that are characterized by the sudden appearance and the (in) satisfaction between the act of buying and the results obtained (Reisch and Zhao, 2017 ). Aragoncillo and Orús ( 2018 ) also refer that a considerable percentage of sales comes from purchases that are not planned and do not correspond to the intended products before entering the store.

According to Burton et al. ( 2018 ), impulse purchases occur when there is a sudden and strong emotional desire, which arises from a reactive behavior that is characterized by low cognitive control. This tendency to buy spontaneously and without reflection can be explained by the immediate gratification it provides to the buyer (Pradhan et al., 2018 ).

Impulsive shopping in addition to having an emotional content can be triggered by several factors, including: the store environment, life satisfaction, self-esteem, and the emotional state of the consumer at that time (Gogoi and Shillong, 2020 ). We believe that impulse purchases can be stimulated by an unexpected need, by a visual stimulus, a promotional campaign and/or by the decrease of the cognitive capacity to evaluate the advantages and disadvantages of that purchase.

The buying experience increasingly depends on the interaction between the person and the point of sale environment, but it is not just the atmosphere that stimulates the impulsive behavior of the consumer. The sensory and psychological factors associated with the type of products, the knowledge about them and brand loyalty, often end up overlapping the importance attributed to the physical environment (Platania et al., 2016 ).

The impulse buying causes an emotional lack of control generated by the conflict between the immediate reward and the negative consequences that the purchase can originate, which can trigger compulsive behaviors that can become chronic and pathological (Pandya and Pandya, 2020 ).

Sohn and Ko ( 2021 ), argue that although all impulse purchases can be considered as unplanned, not all unplanned purchases can be considered impulsive. Unplanned purchases can occur, simply because the consumer needs to purchase a product, but for whatever reason has not been placed on the shopping list in advance. This suggests that unplanned purchases are not necessarily accompanied by the urgent desire that generally characterizes impulse purchases.

The impulse purchases arise from sensory experiences (e.g., store atmosphere, product layout), so purchases made in physical stores tend to be more impulsive than purchases made online. This type of shopping results from the stimulation of the five senses and the internet does not have this capacity, so that online shopping can be less encouraging of impulse purchases than shopping in physical stores (Moreira et al., 2017 ).

Researches developed by Aragoncillo and Orús ( 2018 ) reveal that 40.0% of consumers spend more money than planned, in physical stores compared to 25.0% in online purchases. This situation can be explained by the fact that consumers must wait for the product to be delivered when they buy online and this time interval may make impulse purchases unfeasible.

Following the logic of Platania et al. ( 2017 ) we consider that impulse buying takes socially accepted behavior to the extreme, which makes it difficult to distinguish between normal consumption and pathological consumption. As such, we believe that compulsive buying behavior does not depend only on a single variable, but rather on a combination of sociodemographic, emotional, sensory, genetic, psychological, social, and cultural factors. Personality traits also have an important role in impulse buying. Impulsive buyers have low levels of self-esteem, high levels of anxiety, depression and negative mood and a strong tendency to develop obsessive-compulsive disorders. However, it appears that the degree of uncertainty derived from the pandemic that hit the world and the consequent economic crisis, seems to have changed people's behavior toward a more planned and informed consumption (Sheth, 2020 ).

Author Contributions

All authors listed have made a substantial, direct and intellectual contribution to the work, and approved it for publication.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

  • Aragoncillo L., Orús C. (2018). Impulse buying behaviour: na online-offline comparative and the impact of social media . Spanish J. Market. 22 , 42–62. 10.1108/SJME-03-2018-007 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Burton J., Gollins J., McNeely L., Walls D. (2018). Revisting the relationship between Ad frequency and purchase intentions . J. Advertising Res. 59 , 27–39. 10.2501/JAR-2018-031 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ding Y., DeSarbo W., Hanssens D., Jedidi K., Lynch J., Lehmann D. (2020). The past, present, and future of measurements and methods in marketing analysis . Market. Lett. 31 , 175–186. 10.1007/s11002-020-09527-7 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Falebita O., Ogunlusi C., Adetunji A. (2020). A review of advertising management and its impact on consumer behaviour . Int. J. Agri. Innov. Technol. Global. 1 , 354–374. 10.1504/IJAITG.2020.111885 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gogoi B., Shillong I. (2020). Do impulsive buying influence compulsive buying? Acad. Market. Stud. J. 24 , 1–15. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Khan M., Tanveer A., Zubair S. (2019). Impact of sales promotion on consumer buying behavior: a case of modern trade, Pakistan . Govern. Manag. Rev. 4 , 38–53. Available online at: https://ssrn.com/abstract=3441058 [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kumar A., Chaudhuri S., Bhardwaj A., Mishra P. (2020). Impulse buying and post-purchase regret: a study of shopping behavior for the purchase of grocery products . Int. J. Manag. 11 , 614–624. Available online at: https://ssrn.com/abstract=3786039 [ Google Scholar ]
  • Malter M., Holbrook M., Kahn B., Parker J., Lehmann D. (2020). The past, present, and future of consumer research . Market. Lett. 31 , 137–149. 10.1007/s11002-020-09526-8 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Meena S. (2018). Consumer psychology and marketing . Int. J. Res. Analyt. Rev. 5 , 218–222. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Moreira A., Fortes N., Santiago R. (2017). Influence of sensory stimuli on brand experience, brand equity and purchase intention . J. Bus. Econ. Manag. 18 , 68–83. 10.3846/16111699.2016.1252793 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pandya P., Pandya K. (2020). An empirical study of compulsive buying behaviour of consumers . Alochana Chakra J. 9 , 4102–4114. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Platania M., Platania S., Santisi G. (2016). Entertainment marketing, experiential consumption and consumer behavior: the determinant of choice of wine in the store . Wine Econ. Policy 5 , 87–95. 10.1016/j.wep.2016.10.001 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Platania S., Castellano S., Santisi G., Di Nuovo S. (2017). Correlati di personalità della tendenza allo shopping compulsivo . Giornale Italiano di Psicologia 64 , 137–158. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pradhan D., Israel D., Jena A. (2018). Materialism and compulsive buying behaviour: the role of consumer credit card use and impulse buying . Asia Pacific J. Market. Logist. 30 ,1355–5855. 10.1108/APJML-08-2017-0164 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Reisch L., Zhao M. (2017). Behavioural economics, consumer behaviour and consumer policy: state of the art . Behav. Public Policy 1 , 190–206. 10.1017/bpp.2017.1 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sheth J. (2020). Impact of Covid-19 on consumer behavior: will the old habits return or die? J. Bus. Res. 117 , 280–283. 10.1016/j.jbusres.2020.05.059 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sohn Y., Ko M. (2021). The impact of planned vs. unplanned purchases on subsequent purchase decision making in sequential buying situations . J. Retail. Consumer Servic. 59 , 1–7. 10.1016/j.jretconser.2020.102419 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Stankevich A. (2017). Explaining the consumer decision-making process: critical literature review . J. Int. Bus. Res. Market. 2 , 7–14. 10.18775/jibrm.1849-8558.2015.26.3001 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Varadarajan R. (2020). Customer information resources advantage, marketing strategy and business performance: a market resources based view . Indus. Market. Manag. 89 , 89–97. 10.1016/j.indmarman.2020.03.003 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wertenbroch K., Schrift R., Alba J., Barasch A., Bhattacharjee A., Giesler M., et al.. (2020). Autonomy in consumer choice . Market. Lett. 31 , 429–439. 10.1007/s11002-020-09521-z [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]

Advertisement

Supported by

Why Elon Musk’s OpenAI Lawsuit Leans on A.I. Research From Microsoft

In his lawsuit against OpenAI and its chief executive, Sam Altman, Mr. Musk relies on a provocative paper from the start-up’s closest partner.

  • Share full article

An outside photo of a glassy Microsoft office building.

By Karen Weise and Cade Metz

Karen Weise reported from Seattle, and Cade Metz from San Francisco.

When Elon Musk sued OpenAI and its chief executive, Sam Altman, for breach of contract on Thursday, he turned claims by the start-up’s closest partner, Microsoft, into a weapon.

He repeatedly cited a contentious but highly influential paper written by researchers and top executives at Microsoft about the power of GPT-4, the breakthrough artificial intelligence system OpenAI released last March .

In the “Sparks of A.G.I.” paper, Microsoft’s research lab said that — though it didn’t understand how — GPT-4 had shown “sparks” of “artificial general intelligence,” or A.G.I., a machine that can do everything the human brain can do.

It was a bold claim , and came as the biggest tech companies in the world were racing to introduce A.I. into their own products.

Mr. Musk is turning the paper against OpenAI, saying it showed how OpenAI backtracked on its commitments not to commercialize truly powerful products.

Microsoft and OpenAI declined to comment on the suit. (The New York Times has sued both companies, alleging copyright infringement in the training of GPT-4.) Mr. Musk did not respond to a request for comment.

Draw a unicorn in TiKZ

research papers on buying

How did the research paper come to be?

A team of Microsoft researchers, led by Sébastien Bubeck, a 38-year-old French expatriate and former Princeton professor, started testing an early version of GPT-4 in the fall of 2022, months before the technology was released to the public. Microsoft has committed $13 billion to OpenAI and has negotiated exclusive access to the underlying technologies that power its A.I. systems.

As they chatted with the system, they were amazed. It wrote a complex mathematical proof in the form of a poem, generated computer code that could draw a unicorn and explained the best way to stack a random and eclectic collection of household items. Dr. Bubeck and his fellow researchers began to wonder if they were witnessing a new form of intelligence.

“I started off being very skeptical — and that evolved into a sense of frustration, annoyance, maybe even fear,” said Peter Lee, Microsoft’s head of research. “You think: Where the heck is this coming from?”

What role does the paper play in Mr. Musk’s suit?

Mr. Musk argued that OpenAI had breached its contract because it had agreed to not commercialize any product that its board had considered A.G.I.

“GPT-4 is an A.G.I. algorithm,” Mr. Musk’s lawyers wrote. They said that meant the system never should have been licensed to Microsoft.

Mr. Musk’s complaint repeatedly cited the Sparks paper to argue that GPT-4 was A.G.I. His lawyers said, “Microsoft’s own scientists acknowledge that GPT-4 ‘attains a form of general intelligence,’” and given “the breadth and depth of GPT-4’s capabilities, we believe that it could reasonably be viewed as an early (yet still incomplete) version of an artificial general intelligence (A.G.I.) system.”

How was it received?

The paper has had enormous influence since it was published a week after GPT-4 was released.

Thomas Wolf, co-founder of the high-profile A.I. start-up Hugging Face, wrote on X the next day that the study “had completely mind-blowing examples” of GPT-4.

Microsoft’s research has since been cited by more than 1,500 other papers, according to Google Scholar . It is one of the most cited articles on A.I. in the past five years, according to Semantic Scholar.

It has also faced criticism by experts, including some inside Microsoft, who were worried the 155-page paper supporting the claim lacked rigor and fed an A.I marketing frenzy.

The paper was not peer-reviewed, and its results cannot be reproduced because it was conducted on early versions of GPT-4 that were closely guarded at Microsoft and OpenAI. As the authors noted in the paper, they did not use the GPT-4 version that was later released to the public, so anyone else replicating the experiments would get different results.

Some outside experts said it was not clear whether GPT-4 and similar systems exhibited behavior that was something like human reasoning or common sense.

“When we see a complicated system or machine, we anthropomorphize it; everybody does that — people who are working in the field and people who aren’t,” said Alison Gopnik, a professor at the University of California, Berkeley. “But thinking about this as a constant comparison between A.I. and humans — like some sort of game show competition — is just not the right way to think about it.”

Were there other complaints?

In the paper’s introduction, the authors initially defined “intelligence” by citing a 30-year-old Wall Street Journal opinion piece that, in defending a concept called the Bell Curve, claimed “Jews and East Asians” were more likely to have higher I.Q.s than “blacks and Hispanics.”

Dr. Lee, who is listed as an author on the paper, said in an interview last year that when the researchers were looking to define A.G.I., “we took it from Wikipedia.” He said that when they later learned the Bell Curve connection, “we were really mortified by that and made the change immediately.”

Eric Horvitz, Microsoft’s chief scientist, who was a lead contributor to the paper, wrote in an email that he personally took responsibility for inserting the reference, saying he had seen it referred to in a paper by a co-founder of Google’s DeepMind A.I. lab and had not noticed the racist references. When they learned about it, from a post on X, “we were horrified as we were simply looking for a reasonably broad definition of intelligence from psychologists,” he said.

Is this A.G.I. or not?

When the Microsoft researchers initially wrote the paper, they called it “First Contact With an AGI System.” But some members of the team, including Dr. Horvitz, disagreed with the characterization.

He later told The Times that they were not seeing something he “would call ‘artificial general intelligence’ — but more so glimmers via probes and surprisingly powerful outputs at times.”

GPT-4 is far from doing everything the human brain can do.

In a message sent to OpenAI employees on Friday afternoon that was viewed by The Times, OpenAI’s chief strategy officer, Jason Kwon, explicitly said GPT-4 was not A.G.I.

“It is capable of solving small tasks in many jobs, but the ratio of work done by a human to the work done by GPT-4 in the economy remains staggeringly high,” he wrote. “Importantly, an A.G.I. will be a highly autonomous system capable enough to devise novel solutions to longstanding challenges — GPT-4 can’t do that.”

Still, the paper fueled claims from some researchers and pundits that GPT-4 represented a significant step toward A.G.I. and that companies like Microsoft and OpenAI would continue to improve the technology’s reasoning skills.

The A.I. field is still bitterly divided on how intelligent the technology is today or will be anytime soon. If Mr. Musk gets his way, a jury may settle the argument.

Karen Weise writes about technology and is based in Seattle. Her coverage focuses on Amazon and Microsoft, two of the most powerful companies in America. More about Karen Weise

Cade Metz writes about artificial intelligence, driverless cars, robotics, virtual reality and other emerging areas of technology. More about Cade Metz

IMAGES

  1. (PDF) CONSUMER BUYING DECISION PROCESS TOWARD PRODUCTS

    research papers on buying

  2. research papers on impact of promotional media on consumer buying

    research papers on buying

  3. (PDF) Buy Research Paper

    research papers on buying

  4. Consumer Buying Behavior Free Essay Example

    research papers on buying

  5. (PDF) REVIEW OF LITERATURE: ONLINE AND OFFLINE CONSUMER BUYING BEHAVIOR

    research papers on buying

  6. research paper on consumer buying behaviour

    research papers on buying

VIDEO

  1. buying papers #paper #stationary #printing

  2. Get Paid To Take Part In Research Studies #makemoneyonline2023 #letstalkmoney

  3. Read documents and write research papers with this AI

  4. Best AI tools for finding research papers for your thesis #aitools #researchtips #chatgpt #studytips

  5. How to Write Good Research Papers

  6. The REAL Reason You Procrastinate On Your Research Papers (And How To Fix It TODAY!)

COMMENTS

  1. Factors Affecting Impulse Buying Behavior of Consumers

    The importance of impulse buying in consumer behavior has been studied since the 1940's, since it represents between 40.0 and 80.0% of all purchases. This type of purchase obeys non-rational reasons that are characterized by the sudden appearance and the (in) satisfaction between the act of buying and the results obtained (Reisch and Zhao, 2017).

  2. Impulse buying: A systematic literature review and future research

    Impulse buying research emerged 70 years ago, yet the literature on the topic remained scarce for decades, with just a handful of papers published between 1950 and 2000 (one publication to 3 years ratio). At the beginning of the millennia, impulse buying research experienced steady growth, with 27 papers published between 2000 and 2010.

  3. Impulse buying: a meta-analytic review

    Impulse buying by consumers has received considerable attention in consumer research. The phenomenon is interesting because it is not only prompted by a variety of internal psychological factors but also influenced by external, market-related stimuli. The meta-analysis reported in this article integrates findings from 231 samples and more than 75,000 consumers to extend understanding of the ...

  4. Theory and Models of Consumer Buying Behaviour: A Descriptive Study

    The consumer behavior theory by Schiffman and Kanuk describes the actions that consumers take to find, acquire, use, assess, and discard goods, services, and concepts (Roy, 2022). In other words ...

  5. Consumer Behavior Research: A Synthesis of the Recent Literature

    Inevitably, these changes lead to changed consumer behavior studies by which, when, how, and why the topics are studied. Like any other discipline, systematic analysis of the knowledge development status of consumer behavior field is critical in ensuring its future growth (Williams & Plouffe, 2007).It is of a greater importance for a field of research such as consumer behavior that, as ...

  6. Online shopping: Factors that affect consumer purchasing behaviour

    E-commerce and e-business has been the topic of research for many researches, as until 2013, there were more than 600 studies available discussing e-business adoption only (Chen & Holsapple, 2013). In the growing competition of online stores, it is inevitable to monitor factors that affect potential customers during their buying journey.

  7. Post‐purchase effects of impulse buying: A review and research agenda

    However, if a paper studied both antecedents and outcomes, it was included. Based on this, 51 research articles solely focusing on the pre-purchase stage of the impulse buying process were excluded. In addition, papers on compulsive buying (15) and papers written in languages other than English were excluded (46).

  8. Factors Influencing Online Shopping Behavior: The ...

    Research Methodology The relationship of subjective norm, perceived usefulness and online shopping behavior while mediated by online shopping intention was investigated. The quantitative survey method was conducted by distributing the questionnaires to both the undergraduate and postgraduate students in one higher learning institution in Perlis ...

  9. Impact Of Social Media On Consumer Behaviour

    The goal of this paper is to research empirically the role of social media in consumers' decision-making processes. ... on consumer buying behaviour through surveys and published papers with ...

  10. (PDF) A Review of Impulse Buying Behavior

    The purpose of this paper is to provide a detailed account of the impulse buying behavior by compiling the various research works literature in the field of Retailing and Consumer Behavior. It ...

  11. Theory and Models of Consumer Buying Behaviour: A Descriptive Study

    The study focuses on how consumers decide what to buy and how they make those decisions. The purpose of this study is to comprehend how consumers make judgments about what to buy for personal use. In addition, it explores the basic research on consumer buying behaviour, models of consumer buying behaviour, factors affecting buying behaviour ...

  12. Factors Affecting Consumers' Purchase Behaviour for Health and Wellness

    A conceptual research framework and the research methodology are discussed in the fifth and sixth sections. The result of the analysis and discussion is given in the seventh section of the article followed by the conclusion and managerial implications. Limitations and future research directions are discussed in the last section of this article.

  13. Consumer Buying Behaviour Towards Online and Offline Shopping ...

    Originality/value: The findings of this research provide insight into consumer buying patterns for both online and offline shopping. This research advances our knowledge of both online and offline shopping by providing a conceptual framework for analyzing consumer buying behavior before and after the Covid-19 outbreak.

  14. Impact of Pricing and Product Information on Consumer Buying Behavior

    The behavior of consumers toward buying decisions, the measurement scale of Bagga and Bhatt (2013) is used with slight modification to fit the scale for scope and broaden the view of this research. All predefined models/scales were rated on 5-point Likert scale, with higher numerical values indicating greater satisfaction.

  15. Shop and Discover Books, Journals, Articles and more

    Explore Elsevier's flexible eBook solutions. Open - Buy once and receive all available eBook formats, including PDF, EPUB, and EPUB3. Easy - Download and start reading immediately. There's no activation process to access eBooks; all eBooks are fully searchable and enabled for copying, pasting, and printing. Flexible - Read on multiple ...

  16. Analysis of the Factors Influencing the Consumer Buying ...

    To maintain this steep rise and retain the consumers, there is a need to understand factors that influence the buying behavior of the online consumers. This study is an effort in the same direction. The authors have used questionnaire for collecting primary data from responses focusing on demography, personal and family details and various ...

  17. (Pdf) Consumers' Buying Behavior on Online Shopping: an Utaut and Lum

    Academia.edu is a platform for academics to share research papers. CONSUMERS' BUYING BEHAVIOR ON ONLINE SHOPPING: AN UTAUT AND LUM MODEL APPROACH ... METHODOLOGY A. Research Design This study is a qualitative research about consumer's buying behavior on online shopping. The researchers utilize descriptive research design since its main purpose ...

  18. The Newest Way to Buy an Advantage in College Admissions

    Scholar Launch charges $3,500 for "junior" research programs and between $4,500 and $8,800 for advanced research, according to its website. Polygence, one of the largest online high school ...

  19. Buy Research Papers Online: The 5 Best Sites for College Students

    Maybe, maybe not. But if you are in a crunch and need a professional research paper completed for you as soon as possible, there are some decent writing services that you can depend on. #1 ...

  20. Fake academic papers are on the rise: why they're a danger and how to

    Lex Bouter is the founding chair of the World Conferences on Research Integrity Foundation and co-chair of the 8th WCRI in Athens, 2-5 June 2024. In the 1800s, British colonists in India set about ...

  21. Tax Policy and Investment in a Global Economy

    Tax Policy and Investment in a Global Economy. Gabriel Chodorow-Reich, Matthew Smith, Owen M. Zidar & Eric Zwick. Working Paper 32180. DOI 10.3386/w32180. Issue Date March 2024. We evaluate the 2017 Tax Cuts and Jobs Act. Combining reduced-form estimates from tax data with a global investment model, we estimate responses, identify parameters ...

  22. Research Papers from Professional Writers at EssayPro.com

    Buying a research paper online through EssayPro comes with a 100% guarantee of authentic and original work. We recommend that you submit a piece of your past writing so your writer can get an idea of your voice and writing style. This will help them to match the new essay to sound just like you. Every research paper completed by our expert ...

  23. Buying Research Papers: Is It Legit to Order Them Online?

    How to buy a research paper from a reliable company. When writing a research paper, you will need a good sample with the necessary structure, formatting, and citations. If you buy it from a credible academic help company, you will get an original research paper written according to your demands. Such an example will cut your research time and ...

  24. [2402.17485] EMO: Emote Portrait Alive -- Generating Expressive

    In this work, we tackle the challenge of enhancing the realism and expressiveness in talking head video generation by focusing on the dynamic and nuanced relationship between audio cues and facial movements. We identify the limitations of traditional techniques that often fail to capture the full spectrum of human expressions and the uniqueness of individual facial styles. To address these ...

  25. Consumer Buying Behaviour towards online shopping.

    This paper tries to analyze studies done by various researchers in order to explore what online channels consumers use when they are in a particular stage of the buying decision process (BDP ...

  26. When New Hires Get Paid More, Top Performers Resign First

    Moreover, data from the U.S. Chamber of Commerce indicates that the workforce is expected to shrink in 2024, while a global survey of more than 30,000 employees found that salaries are expected to ...

  27. Why gun violence research was quashed and how it's gaining new momentum

    There is momentum now, in a time of rising gun injury and death, to know more. Gun sales reached record levels in 2019 and 2020. Shootings soared. In 2021, for the second year, more people died ...

  28. Stable Diffusion 3: Research Paper

    Key Takeaways: Today, we're publishing our research paper that dives into the underlying technology powering Stable Diffusion 3. Stable Diffusion 3 outperforms state-of-the-art text-to-image generation systems such as DALL·E 3, Midjourney v6, and Ideogram v1 in typography and prompt adherence, based on human preference evaluations.

  29. Factors Affecting Impulse Buying Behavior of Consumers

    The importance of impulse buying in consumer behavior has been studied since the 1940's, since it represents between 40.0 and 80.0% of all purchases. This type of purchase obeys non-rational reasons that are characterized by the sudden appearance and the (in) satisfaction between the act of buying and the results obtained (Reisch and Zhao, 2017).

  30. Why Elon Musk's OpenAI Lawsuit Leans on A.I. Research From Microsoft

    March 2, 2024, 5:03 a.m. ET. When Elon Musk sued OpenAI and its chief executive, Sam Altman, for breach of contract on Thursday, he turned claims by the start-up's closest partner, Microsoft ...